Anne J. MacCleave
Abstract
What
happens when a clinical nutritionist, an interpretive ethnographer, a grounded
theorist, a statistician/measurement specialist, and poststructuralist feminist
meet to collaborate for a research grant on body image? Excerpts of an
imaginary play about this collaboration will be explored by thinking of
different research traditions as different cultures and researchers who cross
disciplinary boundaries as cultural negotiators. Also explored will be the
languages and assumptions of alternative research traditions and issues of
incommensurabilty. Recommendations are generated to help this fictional
committee submit a coherent proposal and create a mutual space for all to
contribute optimally.
The integrative and
interdisciplinary nature of Family and Consumer Sciences/Human Sciences has
been claimed as a distinguishing feature throughout the field’s history.
However, Vincenti (1990) questioned whether the various specializations in the
field were joined to serve a common purpose or merely for the sake of
convenience. Brown (1993) also lamented the evolution of a “conglomeration of
specializations” that were autonomous and separate (p. 221). Based on the work
of Kockelmans (1979), she questioned the meaning of “interdisciplinary,” noting
that the term was not being used in the genuine sense of creating new patterns
of integrated knowledge.
Perhaps the specializations are autonomous and
separate because they have developed “their own way of doing things, deeply
embedded . . . assumptions and different specialized languages” (MacCleave,
2004, p. 2). One meaning system cannot readily map onto an alternative meaning
system without considerable distortion, incoherence, and confusion (MacCleave,
2003, 2004). Specializations with widely diverse approaches are thus resistant
to “genuine” interdisciplinarity.
Many changes have occurred in research since
Kockelmans created his categories 25 years ago and Brown adapted these for the
field. Among changes are increased promotion of crossdisciplinary research and
increased support for mixed methods and methodologies (Lather, 1992; MacCleave,
2003, 2004; Shulha & Wilson, 2003). Other changes include the advent of
chaos theory, postmoderism along with counterdisciplinary and arts-based modes
of inquiry, and the evolution of ideas in all fields of study (McGregor, 2003;
St Pierre, 2000; Eisner, 1997). Many of these changes have been reflected in
the field of FCS/Human Sciences (See for example, Pendergast, 2001).
Given the nature and extent of specialization in
the field and the evolution of approaches to research, how might research
around a common area of interest be conducted across specialized boundaries,
whether in or outside the field? To explore possibilities for researching
across disciplines and subdisciplines, excerpts of a fictional play will be
analyzed using broad principles of cultural psychology (MacCleave, James, &
Stairs, 2002).
Over the past 18 years, I’ve been teaching an
introductory research seminar course to graduate education students at Mount
Saint Vincent University. This hypothetical play is based on this teaching
experience plus reviewing research grant proposals and articles 1,
conducting research across disciplines, and using alternative methodologies in
my own research. Ideas were also gleaned from the experiences of colleagues and
the research literature. Despite scattered references to actual research, the
fictional researchers and their projects are a product of my imagination.
A Research Story:
Proposing a Research Project Across Disciplines
As Committee Chair, Dr. Amelia Rusch takes her
place at the head of the table. Dr. Rusch is admired for her efficiency in
conducting meetings and her bright and cheery manner. She is an exceedingly
busy professional who is well regarded in her field and highly productive.
Amelia:
I will begin our meeting by welcoming all of you who have decided to join us
for an interdisciplinary research grant on body image from the Health and
Wellness Foundation. Body image is one of the major themes for this year’s
competition given all the recent focus on American overweight and obesity rates
and related health issues. The Foundation is also pushing for more research
across disciplinary boundaries and favors combining qualitative and
quantitative methods. With these ideas in mind, I want to welcome Dr. Ron
Cotton, a qualitative researcher, from the Departments of Sociology and
Clothing and Textiles and Dr. Samuel Froth, a colleague, who is introducing qualitative
methods to our Department of Nutrition Studies. I also want to welcome Dr.
Sandy Post, a Family and Consumer Sciences Educator, and Dr. Jeremy Numbers who
agreed to serve as our statistical consultant.
As you may be aware, my own research is based in
clinical nutrition. My doctoral studies were lab-based experimental
epidemiology studies. However, over the past twelve years, I’ve focused on
issues of body image among adolescents and adults and switched to research that
is largely descriptive and correlational. Recently, I’ve started to focus on
body image with various ethnic populations. So, I’ve accumulated a lot of
empirical data over the years and have become recognized for my contributions
to this area. However, I’m not one to rest on my laurels and was willing to
venture into interdisciplinary work when my Dean recommended joining this
project.
To get the ball rolling, I want each of you, in
turn, to talk briefly about your research background and interests. Also,
please share your major findings in the area of body image to expand our
collection of empirical data. Just add your research to the list I’m
circulating and indicate whether your areas of research have received
extensive, moderate, or low attention. If you cannot add to the list at today’s
meeting, you can fax me your list before our next meeting. That should save a
couple of weeks of delay and we’ll make faster progress on our grant proposal
writing. We might even meet the first of four possible deadlines at the end of
the month—although, that is optimistic! Once we have that knowledge base in
place, we can identify gaps requiring more research and then generate
treatments or interventions that match what the findings are telling us. Let’s
go round the table, starting with you, Ron.
Ron:
I would describe my research background as
“eclectic” and that is reflected in my current position. I have a cross
appointment in the Department of Sociology and the Department of Clothing and
Textiles. My research in the area of self and body image is drawn from an
integration of symbolic interactionism and interpretive ethnography. I’ve
recently expanded this area to include the impact of clothing and adornment on
self/body image—a most interesting perspective, I might add! All of my research
to date has relied upon some version of interpretive or critical modes of
inquiry. I’ll have to check it out, but at this point I’m uncertain if my
research will fit tidily into your list!
Sam: As you know, Amelia,
I’ve moved to the Nutrition Studies Department from the Psychology Department.
I’ve already worked with nutritionists Sarah and Nancy but this will be my
first collaboration with you. I’m a grounded theorist who is introducing this
qualitative approach to your Department. Currently, Sarah, Nancy, and I are
comparing the self-constructed perceptions of body image and health among young
and older adolescents to standardized measures of height, weight, blood tests,
and so on. In the qualitative portion of the study, we are interested in the
complex interplay of subjective, intersubjective, economic, media, and other
sociocultural influences on body image. The hard data part of the study could
certainly be added to your list but, like Ron, I’m uncertain about the other
findings—some adaptation may be in order.
Jeremy: I’m not
directly involved in the study so I don’t have to worry about adding to your
list. I’m here as a statistical consultant or research instrument consultant.
In my department, Education, I’ve developed and statistically validated a
number of research instruments. More recently, I’ve been working on interview
protocols for qualitative researchers in my department and have worked with the
science and math educators to develop qualitative rubrics for classroom
assessments. I’m much more eclectic than most statisticians so may be able to
assist your research team in a number of different ways.
Sandy: I’m a
poststructuralist, feminist researcher and my inquiry into notions of body
image follows from that positionality. I’m especially interested in the
tensions and points of resistance that adult women bring to bear on hegemonic
notions of ideal body image. I want to know how they counter the pervasive
social and cultural messages to forge their own sense of body and self. Unlike
Ron and Sam, I’m totally convinced that my inquiry will NOT fit on your list.
Your research is decidedly structuralist, if not positivist, and I’m
POSTstructuralist, meaning that my inquiries are conducted in a manner that
goes against the grain of most structuralist or traditional approaches to
research.
Amelia: OK, Sandy.
I’ll accept that your approach is different but all the same, you MUST end up
with “findings” of some sort or another or another term that means findings.
Couldn’t these be added to our list?
Sandy: I don’t want my
research appearing like “more of the same” because I’m working hard to build up
a reputation for being counterdisciplinary. Don’t worry. I’ll understand if you
don’t want me on your team. I’ve already warned my Dean that my approach to
inquiry may be considered too “off beat” for this collaboration. It is not that
I am against your research—I’m certain that you are addressing important
concerns. It is just that I need to preserve my difference—after all, it’s my
identity!
Amelia: You might be
surprised, Sandy! After my own experiences of switching research methods, I’m
actually sympathetic towards difference. You wouldn’t believe how appalled my
lab colleagues were when I changed my research focus. They couldn’t imagine why
anyone would want to change from high status, “hard” science to what they
perceived as “fuzzy and soft” science about humans. The irony was that I found
working with humans to be much more challenging than my lab research!
If you’ll bear with us for a while, I’m sure that
we’ll figure out some way that you could contribute to our interdisciplinary
project.
Incommensurability Issues
This research team is as eclectic as one can
imagine! If diversity of perspective was their goal, they have certainly
achieved it. Owing to this diversity, however, the team could easily become
quagmired in issues of incommensurability before making any progress. Simply
stated, incommensurability means that one mode of inquiry cannot be understood
or represented properly using terms that belong to a different mode of inquiry
(MacCleave, 2003, 2004). Amelia seems to be blissfully unaware of different
assumptions underlying alternative research traditions. She did not anticipate
differences in reporting results of qualitative studies compared to quantitative
studies, let alone differences across different versions of qualitative
approaches. To her, these are simply different means of arriving at the same
end, the production of “empirical” data reported as “findings.”
Clearly, Amelia makes no distinctions among
paradigm, methodology, or method. She seems to be operating on the level of
method only while taking her methodological stance for granted.2 In
her own experience, she changed methods from lab-based experimental studies to
working more directly with people using descriptive and correlational methods.
Despite the substantial change in methods, her methodology has remained the
same. These distinctions will be explored further in a later section of this
paper. Included in her methodology are assumptions about what counts as
knowledge and its production (epistemological assumptions) that are not shared
by all members of the team. However, both Sandy and Ron are definitely aware of
these differences. At this stage, it is difficult to fully assess Sam’s or Jeremy’s
position, although both have had eclectic research experiences.
Yet other issues of incommensurability for this
eclectic team are in the area of practice. Amelia normally starts a research
project with a comprehensive literature review, identifies gaps in the research
or areas that have been under-researched, and then makes plans to address these
gaps. This practice is normative for a wide range of research approaches and
disciplines. However, some qualitative researchers intentionally postpone an
extensive literature review until they have drawn their own tentative
conclusions so that sequence of activities cannot be assumed as universal.
Also, when Amelia speaks of generating treatments or interventions, she is
following a medical model. Not all professionals think of their practice in
that way. For example, MacCleave (1999) recalled her shock at educational
interventions being lumped in with other medical or therapeutic interventions.
There may be different understandings among team
members concerning what is meant by researching across disciplines and how the
team ought to proceed. Some may have particular procedures in mind. Others may
come with few preconceived ideas and with the expectation that the process will
be negotiated during the planning phase. Amelia’s use of the term
“interdisciplinary” does not conform to Brown’s definition of creating new
patterns of integrated knowledge. However, Brown (1993) did point out that the
term was used loosely.
Finally, Amelia’s optimistic plan to have a
sound research proposal together in less than a month is totally unrealistic.
With a research team this diverse, much more time and energy would be required
to identify methodological assumptions, sort through methodological issues, and
develop a plan of action that is valuable and coherent. Otherwise, this
research effort might fall prey to “crude eclecticism,” the indiscriminant
mixing of modes of inquiry that have incommensurable or conflicting assumptions
(Brown & Baldwin, 1995). “Full speed ahead” simply will not work in this
situation.
Is There Hope for this Team?
Despite complex interdisciplinary issues to sort
through, this eclectic group of researchers has a number of strengths working
for it. All have prior experience with change or transformation in a research
setting. Although her change was on the level of method, Amelia’s switch from
lab-based to human-centered work was major. Ron transformed his
sociological-based notions of body image to include the role of clothing in its
formation. Sam is expanding the repertoire of research approaches in the
Nutrition Department while adjusting to a biological dimension that is new to
him. Jeremy is adapting methods, if not methodology, to address qualitative
problems. By self-definition, Sandy is all about deconstructing culturally
normative views and finding alternative ways of viewing and portraying reality.
Another strength is that group members do not
appear to be hung up on issues of power and status or the hierarchical thinking
that often characterizes academe (Trubowitz, 2004). Having been on the
receiving end of such appraisals, it is unlikely that Amelia would
intentionally perpetuate such thinking.
Finally, all group members are relatively
open-minded. Although individuals may be unaware of the differing assumptions
underlying their research traditions, they came to the meeting fully expecting
to be introduced to different ways of doing things. Their capacity to grasp the
full meaning of this diversity, however, varies considerably.
Considering the broad-ranging incommensurability
issues facing this research team in combination with their strengths, what are
the possibilities for forging a successful collaboration? The following
questions will guide this inquiry:
What does it mean to
think of research communities as cultures and researchers as cultural
negotiators?
How might one research
community explain itself to the other(s)?
What are the
possibilities for valuing the contributions of often exclusive disciplines and
professions?
How might processes of cultural negotiation contribute to
crossdisciplinary work?
(MacCleave, 2004, p. 7)
Research
Communities as Cultures: Researchers as Cultural Negotiators
MacCleave, James, and Stairs (2002) introduced
basic principles of cultural psychology and discussed implications for FCS.
Also based on principles of cultural psychology, MacCleave (1999, 2004)
depicted different research traditions as different cultural communities and
researchers who crossed disciplinary boundaries as cultural negotiators.3
As cultures, research communities share
Reflection or deliberation about methodological
issues and assumptions may or may not be a normative part of practice within
different research communities.
Not only might the notion of culture be applied
to research communities but it could also apply to researchers. Similar to
educating, crossing research boundaries might be thought of as a dynamic
process of culturing (Stairs, 1996). Researchers might be viewed as
cultural negotiators when they introduce new ways of thinking or acting into
research cultures. Their innovation must be fully understood in relation to the
prior practices of the research community. To what extent are assumptions
underlying the innovation similar or different to existing assumptions within
the research community? MacCleave (2004) related several benefits of
conceptualizing research communities as cultures and researchers who cross disciplines
as cultural negotiators: better understanding of the challenges facing
researchers who cross disciplines; greater appreciation for the value and
accomplishments of these encounters; and enhanced anticipation of possible
outcomes.
Among possible outcomes of working across
disciplines are the following:
Assimilation: The more
dominant or powerful research community assimilates the less dominant one (s).
One danger is that the less dominant research community might lose its
uniqueness, integrity, and purpose. Sandy expressed a fear of this undesirable
outcome in her initial response to Amelia. It was a question of being able to
preserve one’s identity.
Appropriation: At times, one
research culture adopts selected practices from a different research culture.
The adopted practice is simply added to the existing repertoire without
fundamental changes to existing assumptions or practices.
Creation of mutual space:
Mutual space could be created in any number of ways. Researchers could decide
to keep the contributions of different research cultures separate or parallel
but share and learn about each other’s inquiries. They might attempt to
integrate certain portions of the research or even create a whole new cultural
entity. Regardless of how the collaboration is accomplished, the uniqueness and
integrity of the different research cultures would be recognized and valued.
All researchers would be supported in their learning about differences, and all
would find ways to make worthwhile contributions toward a common area of
interest.
Reconceptualizing research across disciplines
and subdisciplines in cultural terms led MacCleave (2003) to rethink the
meaning of incommensurabilty: “I used to think that incommensurability meant
‘contradictory;’ something like claiming at the same time that the world was
round AND flat” (p. 17). However, true contradictions occur only within
particular meaning systems rather than across different systems.4
Incommensurability means that one mode of inquiry cannot be fully understood through
the languages and processes of another mode of inquiry. To understand
incommensurability in this sense, it is important to examine differences in
languages and assumptions across research cultures.
Languages and
Assumptions of Research Cultures
Learning the languages of different research
cultures might be compared to learning foreign languages. It is not uncommon
for those who attempt to learn a new language to initially concentrate on words
or short phrases. They try to substitute a familiar word from their own
language with the equivalent foreign word. When speaking of research findings,
Amelia assumed that if Ron and Sandy didn’t use the term “findings,” then they
must have another term that meant the same thing.
The process of working across disciplines is
complicated by the wide variation in research languages. Sometimes the same
word or terms mean something different across research cultural boundaries. One
example is the word “knowledge.” When Amelia who is schooled in
empirical-analytic/clinical traditions speaks of knowledge, she means something
entirely different from Ron, our interpretive ethnographer, or Sandy, our
poststructural feminist. These differences in meaning are difficult to explain,
let alone understand.
Why do so many different research languages
exist? In cultural terms, alternative research languages might be thought of as
psychological tools (MacCleave, et al., 2002). These specialized languages
serve as a guide for the content and conduct of inquiry and allow the ready
sharing of information and knowledge within a particular research culture. Use
of specialized language among “insiders” of particular research cultures
eliminates the need for elaborate and prolonged explanation. Alternative
research languages might also be thought of as artifacts (MacCleave, et al.,
2002). Among artifacts produced by research cultures are language-based
reports, texts, articles, procedures, and technologies. Although artifacts
evolve over time, current artifacts influence subsequent activity and provide a
historical snapshot of a research culture at a particular point of time.
To return to the “learning a foreign language”
analogy, how does one progress to fluency or more than a superficial
understanding of a new language? To acquire fluency, one must progress beyond
the word or short phrase to grasp the broader, interconnected patterns or
overall system of meaning within which individual words are embedded (Vygotsky,
1962, 1978). When speaking of research cultures, these broad systems of meaning
might be thought of as alternative methodologies. According to Harding (1987), methodology
is “the theory of knowledge or interpretive framework that guides a particular
project” (cited in Lather, 1992, p. 86). In contrast, method refers to
ways of acquiring data or generating knowledge such as structured interviews,
conversational interviews, questionnaires, case study, participant observation,
artifact and document analysis, and autobiographical reflection among others.
Method also encompasses plans for analyzing or interpreting data, reporting
findings, or representing knowledge.5
Some research cultures are familiar with
dialoguing about methodological issues whereas others take their methodology
for granted. Understanding these complex theoretical and philosophical
differences is an enormous challenge and usually requires immersion in the
literature of the research culture, mentorship with experts in the culture, or
extended self-study.
……………………………………………………………………………….
To further illustrate different research
languages and assumptions, let’s return to the discussions of our
“interdisciplinary” research committee. When Amelia suggested that team members
add their research findings to the list that she was circulating, Ron and Sandy
expressed reservations and Sam spoke of the need for adaptations.
Amelia: I’m not going to
ignore your reservations but I confess that I’m struggling to fully understand
them!
Ron: My experiences may
help illustrate some of the challenges facing our team. When I started working
with the Clothing and Textiles Department, several members of that department
were already conducting research on body image from a systems theory
perspective. To test their particular systems theory, they were generating
series of propositional claims or factual claims about “what was the case”
across several research projects. They invited me to add my research to theirs.
However, I generate meaning-based or perspective-based insights from my inquiry
rather than empirically validated propositions or facts. I found that it was
impossible to simply map my meaning-based reality onto their analytical,
objectified reality. My alternative view of knowledge needs to be considered in
its own right, otherwise the quality and depth of insights generated become
distorted or viewed as a sort of wishy-washy, watered down or incorrectly
stated empiricism. The whole issue was further complicated by the fact that
these colleagues knew other ethnographers who did generate propositional claims
from their inquiries. You see, there is enormous variation within my own field!
Amelia: (looking puzzled) This
is all new to me, Ron. Personally, however, I found that working with humans
directly rather than blood tests, urinalysis, and other biomedical indicators
introduced so many more variables that were difficult to identify. Also, these
variables can’t be controlled to the extent that our lab variables could be! I
anticipated that introducing a number of disciplinary views would help us
discover even more variables that influence body image. I’m assuming of course
that your qualitative research is just as rigorous and scientific as more
quantitative versions. At least that is what Sam has been claiming with his
grounded theory approach in our Department.
Sam: That is what I have
been claiming Amelia and I stand by that claim. There is one difference between
my themes and your findings that Ron’s discussion helped me realize and
express. I’m interested in the complex interplay between context and perception
and changes in perception that occur over participants’ lifetimes. Both themes
and theory emerge from participant stories, grounded in context. Themes in my
research do not stand alone free from context, nor can they be generalized
across contexts in the same way that your findings can be. That’s why I
mentioned earlier that some adaptations may need to be made when reporting
those qualitative themes. Nancy, Sarah, and I could make decontextualized or
context-free findings from the biomedical portion of our research, however. The
qualitative portion of the research is just as rigorous and scientific in its
own right as our quantitative work.
Ron: I also think of my
own qualitative and interpretive inquiry as rigorous and systematic but I’m
looking for the meanings that persons ascribe to their experiences and events
in their lives or the meanings that they ascribe to artifacts, whether material
or ideal. The way I’m defining “scientific” is probably different from
the way you define scientific, Amelia.
Amelia: Wow folks! This is
all fascinating but I’m soooo . . . overwhelmed right now. I’m no longer clear
about how we might proceed!
Ron: I have a few ideas to
share about procedure. We needn’t throw out your list altogether, Amelia. It’s
way too much work to waste! I’ll even add to it with findings from the research
projects of my colleagues who are testing systems theory. Their findings are
empirical. Sam can add his biomedical findings and possibly an adapted version
of his qualitative theme. We already know that Sandy won’t be adding to your
list and neither will I add the insights from my own inquiry for reasons
already shared. Part of the solution to our dilemma might be to generate
separate lists or summaries. Sandy can summarize her poststructuralist feminist
inquiries in relation to body image, and I’ll summarize my insights separately.
I’m not sure but Sam may want to have a summary of his situated themes in
addition to what he is adding to your list. At least that way, we won’t be
mixing things that don’t mix.
Amelia: That sounds like
a feasible way to get things started, Ron. Let’s do it! For the time being, I
think that we should stop for lunch.
Although Ron made a valiant attempt to relate
ideas from his inquiry to Amelia’s research and to avoid using too much
specialized language exclusive to his own field, it is doubtful that she could
fully understand his explanation. Trubowitz (2004) noted that “Years of
separate thinking will not yield to an exchange of words” (p. 17). However,
this attempt positions him as a cultural negotiator for this research
committee. Cultural negotiators must first understand pertinent differences
themselves and be able to “code switch” across alternative research languages
and methodologies. To become expert cultural negotiators, researchers must move
beyond recognition of superficial differences to grasp the broad, interrelated,
and coordinated systems of meaning that characterize particular research
cultures (MacCleave, 1999).
All of our committee members have assumed the
role of cultural negotiator within their respective research communities. When
she explained her current research to former lab colleagues, Amelia served as a
cultural negotiator. Because she has conducted research in two different
settings, she understands both cultures deeply. However, methodology was never
an issue when sharing across these cultures. The theoretical and philosophical
underpinnings of her research cultures were simply assumed or
“taken-for-granted.”
Among her assumed methodological assumptions is
Amelia’s definition of science. When she refers to science, she is probably
thinking of some version of empirical-analytic science (Coomer & Hultgren,
1989). In contrast, Brown (1993) referred to a broader meaning of science as
any general understanding produced or constructed in a systematic and reliable
manner that can respond to challenges and questions. Influenced by the writings
of Jurgen Habermas, Brown’s broad definition encompassed cultural sciences
(history and the humanities) and critical science as well as empirical-analytic
science.
Amelia’s talk of identifying and controlling
variables is also characteristic of empirical-analytic sciences. When
identifying and manipulating reality as variables, these variables are
abstracted, considered in isolation from each other, and removed from context.
Many qualitative researchers disagree with this view and want to consider any
given phenomenon in its full complexity (Slavin, 1992). These qualitative
researchers would speak another language based on differing methodological
assumptions.
Because
he has already experienced difference at the level of methodology, Ron does not
take offense to Amelia’s question about being scientific. He’s probably heard
similar concerns from his empirically-based colleagues in the C&T Department.
He understands that she has been listening to an alien discourse and needs to
relate it to something familiar or search for some common ground. He is a good
cultural negotiator because he can empathize with the enormous cognitive
demands entailed in learning about an alternative research culture. Not only
does this situation make intellectual demands but it also makes emotional
demands. The resulting dissonance can be stressful for students and seasoned
researchers alike (Hein, 2004; MacCleave, 2004).
His suggestion that this highly diverse research
team consider generating separate summaries or lists was a good way to get
started without immediately becoming quagmired in incommensurability issues. As
the old saying goes, “Rome wasn’t built in a day.”
……………………………………………………………………….
Not
all differences in research languages and assumptions are at the level of
methodology. Some differences also occur at the level of method. To illustrate
such differences, let’s tune into a lunchtime conversation between Amelia and
Jeremy, our statistical consultant:
Lunch Conversation
Amelia asked Jeremy to meet with her over the
lunch break to discuss the research instruments that she planned to use for her
portion of the research project.
Amelia: I plan to use the
same protocols that Contento uses for her research on body image—oral
interviews, ratings of line drawings. That way, I’ll be able to compare my
findings to hers. I am also adding a 25- item questionnaire designed and
validated by Saunders.6 All the statements tap subjects’ attitudes
about body image using a 5-point Likert scale. For subjects whose first
language is other than English, I plan to train graduate students to administer
the questionnaire orally. I took care to choose instruments that were already
scientifically valid and reliable.
Jeremy: What sample did
Saunders use for her research?
Amelia: Several groups of
undergraduates from different majors attended a Midwestern University. Data
were collected over a four-year period.
Jeremy: I may be
misunderstanding you, Amelia, but I’m hearing you speak of a “validated”
instrument as if it were a “done deal”! Your research sample will be a
different group entirely from Saunders and you are also using the questionnaire
with different ethnic groups, possibly in an adapted way. It is certainly OK to
use the questionnaire but you’ll have to recheck for validity and reliability.
Amelia: You’re kidding!
That all seems a bit much.
Jeremy: The notions of
validity and reliability are not absolute but relative. A questionnaire or test
may be relatively valid for one group but relatively less valid—or entirely
invalid—for another group. For my graduate classes, I use the example of
standardized tests. Imagine that you gave a 10th grade class a
standardized math test that was designed for a 12th grade class.
Would the test give you results that were valid and reliable?
Amelia: Well no, of course
not! Are you saying that Saunders’ questionnaire will be invalid for my
research subjects? This sounds way more complicated than I anticipated It’s the
first time I’ve ever heard of these ideas!
Jeremy: The test example
that I used is the most extreme—just to make the point about validity and
reliability being relative! It is doubtful that Saunder’s questionnaire would
be totally invalid for your group. It’s just standard practice to recheck
validity and reliability whenever a researcher uses a questionnaire with a
different research sample or when a researcher adapts an existing instrument.
Once any changes are made, you’ve essentially created a new instrument. It’s
not as complicated as it sounds—I could run a coefficient alpha, a common test
for reliability, with just one click of a computer key!
The language of validity and reliability
basically mean the same thing to Amelia and Jeremy. However, Jeremy was aware
of slight differences in the way language was used by Amelia within the context
of practice. It is safe to say that Jeremy’s understanding of these terms is
relatively richer and more nuanced than Amelia’s since tests and measurement is
his specialty. He is assuming the role of cultural negotiator in this scenario.
If Amelia decides to follow Jeremy’s suggestion
to recheck validity and reliability of the questionnaire, this situation would
be an example of cultural appropriation. She would be borrowing an idea
from the research culture of educational tests and measurement and applying it
to her own research culture. An extra step would be added to her research
procedure but this addition would not overly disrupt existing practice. Amelia
and Jeremy are “on the same page” in terms of methodology or, at least, their
respective methodologies are compatible enough to negate any worry of
distorting meaning. The repertoire of existing practice in Amelia’s research
culture would be enriched but not transformed in any major way.
This conversation would not be appropriate for
some members of the committee, especially Ron and Sandy. In Sandy’s case,
traditional notions of validity and reliability simply do not apply. Lather’s
discussion of counterdisciplinary notions of ironic, neopragmatic, rhizomatic,
and voluptuous validity certainly underscores this point (Lather, 1994). She
speaks of validity of transgression versus validity of correspondence.
Recommendations for Creating Mutual Space and Moving
Forward with the Grant Proposal
Writing the grant proposal for such a diverse
group of researchers turned out to be more than Amelia had bargained for. She
came to the meeting anticipating differences but not of the type and magnitude
encountered.
The following recommendations have been
generated to help this committee to submit a coherent proposal and create a
mutual space for all to contribute optimally. Some of the recommendations will
be illustrated with bits of conversation extracted from committee members’
ongoing discussions.
Avoid
overgeneralizing, stereotyping, or essentializing alternative research cultures
as much as possible. Amelia came to the meeting with little prior
knowledge of qualitative modes of inquiry. Some initial overgeneralization
could be expected on her part. However, she is also on the receiving end of
stereotyping. Researchers from different qualitative cultures routinely
stereotype those who run statistical analyses.
Amelia: Sam,
you’re calling my research “hard” because it is quantitative and empirical but
my lab colleagues call my research “soft science.” It is their research that is
“hard and rigorous” because they have so much better control over relevant
variables. So what is it? Am I “hard” or am I “soft”? How confusing is that?
When different research cultures are essentialized (a form
of stereotyping), their characteristics are viewed as rigid and static (Stairs,
1996). Similar to other cultures, they are constantly evolving and changing.
However, research cultures evolve from different starting points so it is
useful to know their respective histories.
Become
comfortable with ambiguity and balance dissonance. Those
researchers who belong to cultures where methodological differences are
routinely deliberated become comfortable with the ambiguity of being only
partially understood by others and vice versa. In fact, a degree of dissonance
is both necessary and desirable if learning is to occur. Imagine if team
members simply lumped all of the different approaches together as if one could
be readily substituted for another! Imagine the jumble of Amelia’s empirical
claims mixed with Sam’s situated themes, Ron’s interpretive insights, and
Sandy’s poststructuralist portrayals!
On the other hand, too much dissonance might precipitate an
anxiety-induced paralysis. The key is balance.
Use conceptual
and mediational tools. A range of conceptual and mediational tools might help this team
identify and sort through major methodological differences. They need tools to
help them contrast/compare their approaches to each other.
A good place to start with this committee would
be broad frameworks such as the classification of research as
empirical-analytic, interpretive, or critical (Coomer & Hultgren, 1989). To
avoid overgeneralizing or essentializing, the framework could be further
subdivided as necessary. For example, empirical-analytic could be subdivided
into verificationist or falsificationist, positivist or postpositivist,
experimental or non-experimental, or in some other way.7 Hultgren
(1989) made a useful distinction between interpretive approaches to research
that would be applicable for our research team members. Some interpretive
approaches are rooted in the social sciences and differ somewhat from those
interpretive approaches rooted in philosophy and the humanities. Sam’s grounded
theory would best fit as a social science version of interpretative research
whereas Ron’s inquiry has dimensions of both social science and
humanities-based versions of interpretive research. Ron’s inquiry also has some
critical dimensions because his portrayal of media impact on body image has an
emancipatory intent.
Sandy convinced the group that a
poststructuralist section should be added to the framework. She brought
Lather’s framework of “predict, understand, emancipate, and deconstruct” as
four distinct research orientations and shared some of the reasons that the
fourth section was necessary (Lather, 1992). Sandy’s research also has
emancipatory intent but, in methodological terms, is different from critical
approaches.
In addition to frameworks, other conceptual
tools exist to help researches sort through methodological differences and name
and locate their research approach in relation to others. Sam brought copies of
a cube developed by Langenbach, Aagaard, and Vaughn (1994) to the next meeting.
This cube was designed to distinguish different approaches to research along
three continnums: truth-seeking or perspective seeking ontology, quantitative
and qualitative method, and reform-seeking or status quo-preserving axiology.
Sandy: Oh my! Big boxes
and little boxes. That looks soooo . . . structuralist. No way I could fit my
inquiry on that cube!
At the next meeting, however, Sandy arrived with
a similar looking cube designed by Constas (1998) to distinguish postmodern
research from other versions.
Sandy: I can’t believe
that I’m resorting to a “cube” to explain myself. This cube caused an uproar
among poststructuralist feminists, let me tell you! 8 It brought to
mind a structuralist trying to reign us in, tame us down, and contain us in
little boxes. After joining this group, it occurred to me that the cube, with
all its imperfections, might actually serve as a starting point to explain my
research to a group of structuralists. It’s more your language, at least. Using
this tool, I would locate my inquiry as unbounded versus bounded, idiosyncratic
versus normative. Aaarrrggghhh! I think that I’m allergic to how “dualistic”
these sound—and I’m supposed to be all about deconstructing dualisms!
Sandy is starting to think like a cultural
negotiator. Constas’ cube helps her start with structuralist language and
processes as a bridge or link to understanding the rudiments of her
poststructuralist stance. Unfortunately, she has to suffer through the structuralist
tendency to want to classify everything and to divide everything into binary
opposites such as male/female or nature/culture (Pendergast, 2001).
Expect
different degrees of understanding among team members exposed to alternative
methodologies. All team members are experts in their own areas of this
research project. They may however be what Lave and Wenger (1991) refer to as
legitimate peripheral participants in other areas. For example, Amelia would be
a peripheral participant with Ron and Sandy’s sections of the research and
they, in turn, would be peripheral participants when it comes to her concerns
with establishing validity and reliability for her questionnaire. All members
may learn enough about what each other is doing in order to serve as core
members for subsequent crossdisciplinary projects. A core member needn’t be an
expert in all areas but has enough familiarity with the overall work of the
group to mentor newcomers into its fold.
Generate criteria for assessing the quality of
different research methodologies. A common error that
occurs when mixing alternative methodologies is judging the quality of one
research approach with criteria more suited to a different approach (MacCleave,
2003). To avoid this possibility, our team members discussed how their
respective evaluation criteria would differ:
Amelia: I’m concerned with
developing research instruments that are valid and reliable. I want to be able
to compare my findings to those of others in my field in order to enhance their
generalizability. I want to ensure the non-biased collection of data by
training my interviewers to use standardized protocols.
Sam: I want to follow
the principles of trustworthiness, dependability, confirmability, and
transferability, and I want to share both depth and detail of my participant’s
experiences through generating thick descriptions. I want to use the constant
comparative method rigorously so that the themes generated from participants’
stories are defensible.
Ron: I want my stories to
be plausible. I want my inquiry to throw light on the meanings that persons
construct about their experiences and ideal and material artifacts they
encounter in their lives. I want readers to gain deeper insight
into the situations that I’m narrating. I want to write an evocative narrative
that is coherent and both intellectually and emotionally powerful. I
want readers to realize new possibilities in the otherwise, mundane and
ordinary.
Sandy: I want my inquiry
to help readers understand how persons construct identities and to understand
power in relationships and the impact of power on perspectives. I want my
research to help readers understand perspectives that are situated, partial,
and positioned. I want to portray the complexity of a situation with all its
contradictions and ambiguities. I want my inquiry to deconstruct binaries and
to unsettle sedimented meanings!
Nurture
cultural negotiators. Researchers should be encouraged to assume the role
of cultural negotiators when introducing their research methodology to others.
Cultural negotiators should be highly valued members of crossdisciplinary teams
and supported in this role. In a similar vein, Brown (1993) promoted the role
of generalists as integrationists for the field of FCS/Human Sciences. The role
of cultural negotiator, integrationist, or crossdisciplinarian needs to be
valued as much as that of the specialist. Such a role requires more years of
study and is intellectually demanding.
Celebrate
transformations in a community of learners. Productive transformations can
and do occur when different research cultures collaborate around an area of
interest, provided that all members are open-minded and willing to learn. It
helps if all parties think of themselves collectively as a community of
learners (Rogoff, 1994). In such a community, assimilation of other cultures or
converting everyone to one way of thinking would be avoided. Rather, the
richness and diversity of plural traditions would be embraced. Members would be
encouraged to learn as much as possible about each other’s approaches but
expertise in all areas would not be expected.
To illustrate productive transformations, Ron
shared the following example from his prior research experiences:
Ron: When I was working
exclusively in the Sociology Department, I explored the impact of the media and
other social and cultural conditions on adolescent and adult women’s
self-constructed notions of body image. I saw media as a predominantly negative
influence and one that led to negative body image for my participants. My view
changed when I started to collaborate with the C & T folks. Even though my
research approach differs from that of my systems colleagues 9, they
helped me consider the positive dimensions of clothing and adornment—the basic
physical, social, and psychological needs that these serve. I had never thought
of body image in that way before!
The C & T folks also learned from my research.
In one of my inquiries, I generated stories of two adolescents from the same
clique who marked group membership with the choice of specific apparel items
worn in a certain way. One young woman, who was slightly insecure, was rigid
about following “the code.” A second adolescent, who was creative and
sensitive, routinely befriended persons outside the group. She was conflicted
about the exclusiveness of the group. Sometimes she conformed to the clique’s
code but other times she resisted it. Owing to all the twists and turns in the
story, my C & T colleagues reexamined one of their questionnaires designed
to distinguish conformity from individuality in clothing choice. Before reading
my study, they often eliminated ambiguous respondents from their data set,
wanting to compare those who were clearly conforming or individuating in
clothing choice. They decided to interview ambiguous respondents in order to
revise their questionnaire. Reality turned out to be more complex that they had
anticipated.
Sandy: If I were
conducting your inquiry, Ron, I’d intentionally seek out the non-conformists.
I’d want to hear the voices of those on the margins. Further, I’d try to
deconstruct the dualism of conforming/non-conforming!
The team decided to meet weekly to read and
discuss selected examples of each other’s research. They used the
empirical-analytic, interpretive, critical, and poststructuralist framework and
the cubes to locate and name their own and each other’s research. They
documented “shared understandings, misunderstandings, points of confusion,
dissonance, and surprise” (MacCleave, 2004, p. 23). This work helped them
develop as cultural negotiators.
For the grant proposal, they generated a series
of research questions from different methodological perspectives, allowing each
researcher to conduct their respective segments relatively independently. To
avoid assessing different segments with inappropriate criteria, each team
member generated a list of criteria for evaluating their research segment. This
project would be classified as crossdisciplinary research. Attempting to
develop genuine interdisciplinary research would be premature for this group.
If they continued to work together, the researchers might attempt to integrate
portions of the research but some of the methodologies are so different they
simply wouldn’t mix well.
After hearing about Ron’s experiences with the C
& T Department, team members decided to translate each other’s inquiries
into questions suitable to their own methodological stance. Their intent,
however, was not to gloss over substantive methodological differences. This
step would encourage the generation of new ideas for future research. Ron and
Jeremy planned to attempt a dialogue across the different research segments
raising questions and noting broad connections or disconnections as a finale
for this crossdisciplinary effort with input from the other team members.
Eight
Months Later: A Happy Ending
Amelia: (beaming) I want to
thank all of you for your wonderful contributions to our research grant
proposal. I can’t tell you how gratified I am that our bid was successful. In
all honesty, I questioned my sanity for starting with this wildly diverse
group, let alone persisting. Many times, I felt that I’d landed on Mars!
Perhaps, that is why this victory is so sweet.
Let me share some of the reviewers’ comments. One
reviewer said: “In all my years reviewing proposals, I’ve yet to encounter a
team that went to such lengths to sort out methodological differences and to
address issues of incommensurability. Your documentation of this work from your
numerous proposal meetings and thoughtful, insightful rationale for procedures
selected were outstanding! That documentation served as an inquiry about the
inquiry. I hope to see more of that sort of thing in your actual research.”
Another reviewer
stated: “I loved the idea of translating each other’s methodological perspective
into questions that suited one’s own perspective. What a terrific way to share
ideas and become engaged in alternative inquiries without necessarily
annihilating each other in the process. Your description of your diverse
methodologies as complementary but incommensurable was most appropriate
for your crossdisciplinary project.”
Amelia continues reading reviewers’ comments.
Amelia: Now the real fun
begins of actually conducting our respective research segments. Let’s first
book our weekly “share and learn” meetings, then our bi-monthly research
progress meetings.
Notes
1.
The topic of body image was inspired by my review of an award winning
undergraduate research paper for the CHEA Foundation. I also knew that this
topic was researched from a number of different disciplinary perspectives. I
taught about body image within the broader topic of physical development in an
introductory course to educational psychology. Based on workshops and puppet
shows with elementary students, I explored the idea of addressing body image
using puppets and costumes as a medium (MacCleave, 2002; MacCleave &
MacCleave, 2001).
2.
Working at the level of method is not uncommon across several disciplines. For
example, Slife and Williams (1997) noted that psychology was primarily
committed to method with relatively less attention to theorizing.
3.
Principles of cultural psychology were drawn from the work of several theorists
including Bruner (1990, 1996); Cole (1996); Geertz (1973); Lave & Wenger
(1991); Rogoff (1994, 2003); Shweder (1990); Smith (1995, 2001); Stairs (1996);
Wenger (1999).
4.
With this view of incommensurability, I take a slight departure from Lincoln
and Guba (1998). They claimed that “commensurability is an issue only when
researchers want to ‘pick and choose’ among the axioms of positivist and
interpretivist models, because the axioms are contradictory and mutually
exclusive” (p. 174). The axioms may be conflicting and may not mix well but
since taking a cultural turn, I now take greater care with claims of “contradiction.”
5.
This definition of method is an expanded version of Harding’s original
definition as “techniques for gathering empirical evidence” (cited in Lather,
1992, p. 86). I found Harding’s definition of method too narrow but found her
use of “methodology” as an intermediate link between paradigm and method to be
helpful (MacCleave, 2000).
6.
The reference to Contento is based on an article by Contento, Basch, and Zybert
(2003). The reference to Saunders and her questionnaire is entirely fictional.
7.
Researchers in the social sciences and humanities often think of natural
science as monolithic. Yet, Hacking (1983) addressed the ongoing methodological
disputes among verificationists and falsificationists plus constructivists and
realists in the natural sciences.
8.
As one example, see the response of St. Pierre (2000) to Constas’ article.
9.
For an example of clothing research from a systems perspective, see Sontag,
Peteu, and Lee (1997).
References
Brown, M. (1993). Philosophical
studies in home economics in the United States: Basic ideas by which home
economists understand themselves. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State
University.
Brown, M., & Baldwin, E.
(1995). The concept of theory in home economics: A philosophical dialogue.
East Lansing, MI: Kappa Omicron Nu Honor Society.
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts
of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. (1996). The
culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural
psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press.
Constas, M. A. (1998). Deciphering
postmodern educational research. Educational Researcher, 27(9),
36-42.
Contento, I. R., Basch, C.,
& Zybert, P. (2003). Body image, weight, and food choices of Latino women
and their young children. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 35,
236-248.
Coomer, D. L., &
Hultgren, H. H. (1989). Considering alternatives: An invitation to dialogue and
question. In F. H. Hultgren, & D. L. Coomer (Eds.). Alternative modes of
inquiry in home economics research (pp. 37-59). Yearbook of the Teacher
Education Section. Washington, D.C.: American Home Economics Association.
Eisner, E. W. (1997). The
enlightened eye: Qualitative inquiry and the enhancement of educational
practice (2nd ed.). New York: Merrill.
Geertz, C. (1973). The
interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.
Hacking, I. (1983).
Representing and intervening: Introductory topics in the philosophy of natural
science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Harding, S. (Ed.). (1987). Feminism
and methodology. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Hein, S. F. (2004). “I don’t
like ambiguity” An exploration of students’ experiences during a qualitative
methods course. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 50(1),
22-38.
Hultgren, H. H. (1989).
Introduction to interpretive inquiry. In F. H. Hultgren, & D. L. Coomer
(Eds.). Alternative modes of inquiry in home economics research (pp.
37-59). Yearbook of the Teacher Education Section. Washington, D.C.: American
Home Economics Association.
Kockelmans, J. J. (1979).
(Ed.). Interdisciplinarity and higher education. University Park:
Pennsylvania University Press.
Langenbach, M., Vaughn, C.,
& Aagaard, L. (1994). An introduction to educational research.
Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Lather, P. (1992). Critical
frames in educational research: Feminist and poststructural perspectives. Theory
into Practice, 31(2), 87-99.
Lather, P. (1994). Fertile
obsession: Validity after poststructuralism. In A. Gitlin (Ed.). Power and
method: Political activism and educational research. New York: Routledge.
Lave, J., & Wenger, E.
(1991). Situated learning. Legitimate peripheral participation.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba,
E. G. (1998). Paradigmatic controversies, contradictions and emerging
confluences. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.). Handbook of
qualitative research (2nd ed.), (pp. 163-189). Thousand Oakes,
CA: Sage.
MacCleave,
A. (1999, April). Integrative research: Dreaming through the lens of
cultural negotiation. Paper presented as part of a symposium (Cultural
Negotiation, Education, Institutions, Communities) at the American
Educational Research Association Annual Meeting, Montreal, P.Q.
MacCleave,
A. (2000, November). Methodology versus method: More than a “nit-picking”
distinction. Abstracts of the Tenth Annual Conference of Atlantic Educators.
Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, N.S.
MacCleave,
A. (2002, July). Exploring human development themes with puppets and
costumes. Performance/paper presented at the Pi Lambda Theta Region 1
Conference, Pittsburgh, PA.
MacCleave,
A. (2003, May). The issue of incommensurability when mixing methods and
methodologies: A reflection on two studies. Paper presented at the XXXI
Annual Conference of the Canadian Society for Studies in Education, Dalhousie
University, Halifax, Nova Scotia.
MacCleave,
A. (2004, May). Incommensurabilty in cross-disciplinary research: A case for
cultural negotiation. Paper presented at the XXXII Annual Conference of the
Canadian Society for Studies in Education, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg,
Manitoba.
MacCleave,
A., James, O., & Stairs, A. (2002). Sociocultural diversity: Insights from
cultural psychology for family and consumer sciences. Kappa Omicron Nu Forum,
3(2) (non-paginated document).
MacCleave,
A., & MacCleave, J. (2001, November). Learning, making and becoming:
Puppets and costumes as a medium. Performance/paper presented at the
Eleventh Annual Conference of Atlantic Educators, Acadia University, Wolfville,
Nova Scotia.
McGregor,
S. L. T. (2003). Postmoderism, consumerism and a culture of
peace. Kappa Omicron Nu Forum, 13(2). Accessed
March 30, 2005 at /archives/forum/13
2/mcgregor.html.
Pendergast,
D. (2001). Virginal mothers, groovy chicks & blokey blokes: Rethinking
home economics and teaching bodies. Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.
Rogoff,
B. (1994). Developing understanding of the idea of community of learners. Mind,
Culture and Activity, 1(4), 209-229.
Rogoff,
B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Slavin,
R. E. (1992). Research methods in education (2nd ed.).
Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Slife,
B. D., & Williams, R. N. (1997). Toward a theoretical psychology: Should a
subdiscipline be formally recognized? American Psychologist, 52(2),
117-129.
Shulha,
L. M., & Wilson, R. J. (2003, May). Collaboration and mixed method
inquiry: Theory and practice. Paper presented at the XXXI Annual Conference
of the Canadian Society for Studies in Education, Dalhousie University,
Halifax, Nova Scotia.
Shweder,
R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – what is it? In J. W. Stigler, R.A. Shweder,
& G. Herdt (Eds.). Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human
development (pp. 1-14). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Smith,
H. A. (1995). Cultural psychology and semiotics: Confronting meaning in
educational practice. Canadian Journal of Education, 20(4),
407-414.
Smith,
H. A. (2001). Psychosemiotics. New York: Peter Lang.
Sontag,
M. S., Peteu, M., & Lee, J. (1997). Relationships among values, proximity
of clothing to self, clothing interest, anticipated outcomes and perceived
quality of life. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 9(4), 41-46.
St
Pierre, E. A. (2000). The call for intelligibility in postmodern educational
research. Educational Researcher, 29(5), 25-28.
Stairs,
A. (1996). Human development as cultural negotiation: Indigenous lessons on
becoming a teacher. Journal of Educational Thought, 30(3),
219-237.
Trubowitz,
S. (2004). The marriage of liberal arts departments and schools of education. Educational
Horizons, 82(2), 114-117.
Vincenti,
V. (1990). Home economics in higher education: Communities of convenience or
purpose? Home Economics Research Journal, 19(2), 184-193.
Vygotsky,
L.S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vygotsky,
L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological
processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wenger,
E. (1999). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning and identity.
Cambridge, UK. Cambridge University Press.
Dr. Anne MacCleave is an Associate Professor in
the Faculty of Education, Mount Saint Vincent University, Halifax, NS, Canada.