Exploring Opportunities for Eco-sound
Food Habits: Households and Researchers in Partnership
Helena Shanahan, Annika Carlsson-Kanyama,
Marianne Pipping Ekström
Dr. Shanahan is an associate professor in the
Department of Home Economics, Göteborg University, Sweden.
Dr. Carlsson-Kanyama is a research leader at
the Environmental Strategies Research Group in Stockhom, Sweden.
Dr. Ekström is an associate professor at
the Department of Home Economics, Göteborg University, Sweden.
Abstract
The purpose of this article is to report selected findings
and highlight the methodological approach used in a Swedish project dealing with food habits in households and their
environmental impacts. The approach is human ecological, longitudinal, and participatory. The households took part in
an experiment with the aim of reducing life cycle energy inputs for food consumed. The article covers a case study of
one of the participating households, describing food management processes, experiences during the experiment, and
outcomes over time. In this household, food habits were changed toward more environmentally friendly ones.
Introduction
Today there is deep concern over environmental
degradation, one of the greatest problems of our time (UNEP, 2001). Consumption patterns must be changed, especially in
the richer parts of the world (OECD, 2001). As the demand for ecologically rational behaviour grows, the
"green" household is seen as a necessary collaborator. It has been estimated that Swedish households cause
almost half of the total environmental degradation in Sweden. Food habits are of particular interest since they
represent the type of consumption in everyday life that impacts most on the environment. In converting household
consumption into energy use, food may count for up to a third of the total. This figure is based on the energy used in
the whole life cycle of foods, from production to preparation. Therefore considerable environmental gains could be
achieved through changing food habits.1 The project2 reported in this paper addresses this issue
by studying food management in households in terms of environmental impact. During a period of three years we worked in
close collaboration with ten households in order to document food-related activities, food consumed, and life cycle
energy inputs. Moreover, the households participated in an experiment in which they tried to reduce energy inputs
related to their food consumption. The purpose of this paper is to report some selected findings from the project as
well as highlight the methodological approach.
Swedish households and environmental
behaviour
Bennulf and Gilljam (1991) have studied behaviour
related to environmental issues. The actions analysed over the years 1990 to 1996 were purchase of green-labeled foods,
separation of household waste, and walking or biking instead of using motor-driven transport. The number of people
choosing to walk or bike instead of using private or public motorised transport remained about the same. On the other
hand, the number who chose to purchase green-labeled foods and sort household waste had risen sharply. However, it is
interesting to note that the number who found environmental degradation the most important problem in the Swedish
society declined dramatically during the same period. Other issues mainly related to the economic situation in the
country were topping the list instead.
Swedish food consumption patterns are changing,
some in the direction of being more environmentally friendly, others not (Carlsson-Kanyama & Lindén, 2001).
Compared to four decades ago, Swedes eat more meat, vegetables, fruit, rice, pasta, candy, and soft drinks and less
potatoes, milk, flour, and legumes. Consumption of high environmental impact foods increased, for example meat and
cheese. Consumption of cereals has also increased: mainly rice, mixed cereals, and pasta. The consumption of coffee,
mineral water, and beer decreased while the consumption of juices increased. In 1999, 13% of Swedish households bought
organic milk. A food consumption study found that low fat and high fibre labeled foodstuffs often were confused with
foods labeled organic. Magnussson et al. found that the most important reason for choosing foods was food taste, and
the least important "organically produced.
Availability of eco-sound foods
The number of organic food products available on
the Swedish market has risen considerably as has the number of certified restaurants serving meals from organically
produced products. Nevertheless, the total market share of eco-labeled food products was less than 1%. In 2000, 5.3% of
the arable land in Sweden was certified according to organic standards. Ten years previously it was only 1%.
Locally produced foods can be purchased at farm
markets, still uncommon in Sweden, and in farm shops outside the cities. Acquiring locally produced foods, therefore,
usually involves considerable effort compared to normal food shopping and is still marginal among Swedish consumers.
However, it is expected that locally produced foods will increase in market share.
Consumers are increasingly requesting information
related to eco-sound foods. In a recent survey among Nordic consumers it was found that almost 90% wanted to know from
which country the product originated and about half wanted information on ethical issues, environment, and animal
welfare. Swedish law stipulates that information on the place of origin of foodstuffs shall be indicated if failure to
give this information might mislead the consumer (Swedish National Food Administration, 2001). However, it is only
mandatory for fresh and frozen meat as well as fresh and processed fish.
Changing everyday habits
How to make everyday behaviour more
environmentally friendly has been on the agenda for nearly half a century and much has been learnt. Both external and
internal barriers must be overcome. For example, in relation to organic food products, external structural conditions
such as high prices and limited supply must be changed. But also internal barriers such as lack of knowledge and
commitment must be overcome. A change can first be expected when the barriers to action are low enough for inexpensive
actions that are ready at hand. There has been an optimistic belief in the use of information in the hope of fostering
environmentally sound behaviour among the general public. However, it has been found that there is no automatic link
between knowing and doing. Norms, values, and feelings have been shown to be a vital part of the picture (Lundgren,
2000).
Models for describing and explaining how
environmentally friendly habits are formed have become more varied. The early models of cause and effect, which showed
that increased knowledge about a phenomenon leads to positive attitudes, and in turn to readiness to act and finally to
changed behaviour, are increasingly challenged. Increased knowledge about how everyday habits impact the environment
can lead to positive attitudes toward green habits, which can in turn be manifested in green behaviour. However,
awareness or knowledge about environmental degradation does not necessarily lead to positive attitudes, and positive
attitudes do not necessarily lead to green behaviour. The importance of studying behaviour in its everyday context has
also been stressed. The focus on individual consumers, detached from their everyday living, is still the most
frequently used approach in consumer behaviour research, as well as models building on causal relationships.
It has also been shown that carrying out green
activities in itself can lead to new knowledge and changed attitudes. Ölander and Thøgersen (1995) call
this experience effect. They argue that the people involved, for example, in a waste source-separation
programme can acquire personal knowledge and replace prejudice and scepticism with knowledge leading to the
establishment of new habits. An early model for explaining behavioural change that has been shown to be useful in
explaining the change to environmentally friendly behaviour is the theory of cognitive dissonance as developed by
Festinger in 1957 (Gardner & Stern, 1996). The essence of this theory is that a person who knows that he or she
does not behave the way the society expects sooner or later gives in to get peace of mind.
Feedback, directly tied to peoples own
behaviour, has been shown to be more effective than general information and prompts. For example, in the 1970s
psychologists started to experiment with methods that offered people regular, usually daily, information about how much
energy they were using, instead of telling people to save energy. Electronic monitoring devices have been particularly
appreciated. An experiment with written feedback on waste generated, given individually to households in connection
with billing and attractively designed, showed to be a contributing factor in reducing waste weights and maintaining
weights at low levels.
In the last couple of decades, eco-labeling has
become a popular environmental policy instrument in countries all over the world for helping consumers to choose green
products. Few schemes have been sufficiently evaluated, but it can be concluded that it takes time and committed effort
to build eco-labeling success. Receptiveness depends on consumer environmental concern, the clarity of the labels
profile, the intensity of its promotion, and its presentation in the shops.
The project
The explorative and longitudinal project,
Foods and the Environment: A Household Ecological Approach, was started in 1998 to study food management
in households related to environmental impact. The project's approach is action and participatory research.
Figure 1 describes four evolving phases. The overall aim of the first phase was to describe management processes
covering the whole chain of events from shopping, transporting, preparing, serving, cleaning up, and waste handling. In
the second phase the environmental impact of the households food habits was analysed. The analysis was made in
terms of life cycle energy inputs for food consumed. In the third phase feedback was given to the households about the
life cycle energy inputs related to food habits. Moreover, a manual for low energy foods habits was developed as a
guideline for the households. Based on this information, the households planned and implemented food habits demanding
less energy. The effect of the participation in this experiment was evaluated in phase four.
Figure 1:The research process
A human ecological approach
Sontag and Bubolzs approach and methodology
when studying families adaptation to a new living situation as part-time farmers inspired the research team. Our
objectives were to study the interaction between the households and systems in the environment such as food markets,
waste management and transport, and social systems. Our project also included the study of the households impact
on the natural environment. Equally important was to explore the dynamic of family life related to resource management.
We have, however, not included the household's children per se in the study. Thus, our project focuses, as did Sontag
and Bubolzs, on interrelationships and the complexity of resource management processes in households. A human
ecological perspective seems particularly appropriate to use. It is holistic, and recognises complexity and
interdependence, multidimensionality, and human consciousness. It is in sharp contrast to the positivistic paradigm
that narrows and fragments everyday life.
Capturing everyday life
To study everyday life in households requires
specific approaches and tools. The design of the project was that of a multiple-case study, with the focus on the
household and with food management processes as an embedded unit of analysis. The households selected for the project
had very similar characteristics in terms of size, age, and socio-economic status and resided in the same area.
Information is needed from a great number of variables when using an ecological approach focusing on the connective
relationship between human action and contextual setting. The greater the number of variables studied, the better the
chance of obtaining an ecologically valid picture of the everyday reality in the household. A grounding for action
research requires systemic thinking in creating the mode that keeps people in touch with the wholeness of our existence
in order to broaden action and deepen research (Reason & Bradbury, 2001). It is also of crucial importance to study
household activities in their natural, everyday settings (Tuomi-Gröhn & Palojoki). Central to action research
is the implementation of experiments in the field rather then in the laboratory (Reason & Bradbury). In line with
this tradition practical outcomes are also important (Engberg, 2000). All this was built into the design of our
project. Many variables were studied; the households participated actively in the gathering of data in addition to
carrying out an experiment in their homes with concrete practical outcomes.
Table 1 summarises the different research methods
that were used for different themes in the project. The sources from which information was gathered are also
shown.
Table 1 Summary of research methods and
themes
|
|
|
Information from
|
Methods |
Themes |
Individual
|
Household
|
Environment
|
|
Assessment |
Attitudes to food |
x
|
|
|
|
Environmental behaviour |
|
x
|
|
|
Decision-making |
x
|
|
|
|
Resource exchange |
|
x
|
|
|
Records |
Food purchases |
x
|
|
|
|
Meals served in the household |
|
x
|
|
|
Meals served outside |
x
|
|
|
|
Waste generated |
|
x
|
|
|
Time/place |
x
|
|
|
|
Transport |
|
x
|
|
|
Interviews |
|
x
|
x
|
|
|
Inventories |
Supermarkets |
|
|
x
|
|
Life cycle analysis |
Environmental impact of food |
|
x
|
x
|
|
|
|
|
|
Checklists were used mainly as assessment
instruments. These were filled in individually by the adult household members. This facilitated comparisons between men
and women. In order to study resource exchange between the household and socio-economic systems in the environment, the
households were asked to draw so called eco-maps.
The households kept a number of records. In order
to document food eaten in the household, the households kept diaries, based on Ekström and Shanahan, Jonsson, and
Renström. These covered time, number of portions, food and drink, storage, and preparation methods. Meals eaten
outside the household were also registered. During the same period, the households also weighed their waste, whether or
not they had separated it into different types (such as paper, glass, organic household waste). Moreover, receipts were
kept in order to record food purchases. Shopping receipts usually indicate if the product is green-labeled. When no
receipt was obtained purchases were recorded separately. Time, activities, place, social interaction, and transport
were recorded for one week by a time-geographic method. The adult members of the households recorded in small
open notebooks when, where, and with whom they carried out activities.
The environmental impacts of food habits were
analysed by estimating life cycle energy inputs (Carlsson-Kanyama, 1999). In-depth interviews, having the character of
informal talks, were carried out on three occasions with adult members of each household in their home, sometimes with
children joining in. Interviews were transcribed in full. We did not ask the households to record income and expenses
since we feel these are very sensitive questions to ask. However, knowing their occupations, we were able to roughly
estimate their incomes.
The experiment and feedback
Based on the data gathered and discussions with
the households, a manual was developed for reducing life cycle energy inputs of food consumed in the households. The
manual contained a checklist for helping the households to plan low energy input food,3 examples of meals
with high and low inputs based on the households own registrations, and a collection of recipes for low energy
dishes. The experiment required the households to try to lower energy inputs for meals served at home over one month.
They kept a food diary for two weeks covering types of food and place of origin. If eco-labeled, number of portions and
preparation methods were recorded. They also listed meals eaten outside the home, weighed their waste, and saved
receipts of food purchases.
Immediately after the experiment, an interview
was held with each household focusing on their experiences in implementing it. In a questionnaire, the households were
asked about the preferred method of feedback: a visit in their home, a joint meeting with the researchers, or a written
report. Most preferred a written report, and this was sent to each household after about half a year. The report gave
feedback on weights of food consumed and the life cycle energy inputs before and during the experiment. Each report
also contained advice to the individual household about how to cut down energy input in their diet. Finally, the
households were interviewed after a year, in late 2001, with the purpose of exploring the long terms effects of the
experiment.
The Andersson family
In this paper we report some selected findings
from our work together with one of the ten households, the Andersson family.4 The reason for choosing this
household is to describe a case showing how change can come about. When we first met them, Sven and his wife Britta
were in their mid 30s, and their children Frida and Simon were 7 and 4 years old respectively. Besides their jobs, the
busy, everyday life of the adults was filled with household chores, childrens activities, union work, and fitness
training. Friends and neighbours and their children came and went. The parents both had shift work, being arranged so
that one parent stayed at home while the other one worked. Frida spent most of her day in school, Simon in a child-care
centre. Sven and Britta each have a university degree, and both work full time. They live in one of the large cities on
the west coast of Sweden. In 1998 they lived in a downtown apartment, but in 2000 they had moved to a suburb, about 5
km away, to live in a terrace house with a garden. The family continued to do the main part of their food shopping in
the same supermarket they had frequented before moving. The family owns a car but Sven and Britta usually bike to work.
After moving they were still able to bike to work, since good biking tracks pass close to the house and conveniently
lead to work in the city. The family also owns a summer cottage.
During the first phases of the project Britta and
Sven seemed to be interested but ambivalent to carrying out environmentally friendly actions in everyday life. In the
first interview they expressed a rather erratic behaviour. In 1998 the Anderssons reported that they always disposed of
glass bottles for recycling but seldom or never separated other types of waste. They also often avoided taking the car
to work or school. They reported that they often bought green-labeled washing powder. However, when it came to buying
green-labeled food products Sven was very suspicious about what they really stand for and how they are controlled. He
said he did not believe in stamps (labels), and he thought that prices were too high. Nevertheless they
bought six different green products during the two-week period when they initially recorded their food purchases.
Britta was more positive toward choosing green-labeled foodstuffs. Both of them expressed interest in developing the
habit of buying green products, which they felt was important from a moral viewpoint and in educating their children
for the future. However, for the time being they were hesitant. When they were discussing buying eco-labeled foods they
seemed to misunderstand each other and there was a miss-match in their communication. They argued about buying
eco-labeled coffee. Britta resented that Sven was negative to buying green foods but she was not consistent in her own
behaviour.
When it came to the Municipalitys role in
recycling waste Sven was again very suspicious and disappointed. He said he was paranoid and related what
he had observed when emptying the glass igloos, one for coloured and one for non-coloured/white
glass.
First they empty the white glass and
then the next one is emptying everything (the coloured glass) on top of it. I asked: Why you do it like
that? We dont have possibilities to take care of it anyway. All shit ends up at the same place.
Then my faith is disappearing. I dont have any faith in that at all.
Britta pointed out that waste recycling did not
work in the city but that it did in other places. Sven thought separation of waste into different types and disposal at
the recycling station were too time consuming. But because it was important for Britta, he did it most of the
time.
When she is there bossing me around,
Ill do it. But when Britta is not at home the rubbish will end up in the same place. Unfortunately thats
the way it is! I would very much like to feel that what Im doing the others in the system do too, so that it
really would be done.
Nevertheless, when the household reported waste
weights, they reported five different types. Previously they also used to separate organic waste and put it in the
compostor in the yard but had stopped doing so. Also here they had different opinions. Britta thought it worked well,
Sven did not. He would like the Municipality also to pick up separated organic waste. During the week they weighed
their waste, the total amount was 20 kg. This figure corresponds to findings from other studies of an average of 5.7 kg
per week and person for households in apartments.
Living through the experiment
Sven and Britta claimed that during the
experiment they had lived just about normally. They found it a positive experience having to raise their awareness
about the daily activities related to food habits. On the other hand they found it difficult to follow the advice to
plan what to eat and shop in advance. Their strategy had always been to plan when walking around in the
shop. They found it difficult to find information about where foods had been produced. Sven said:
. . . sometimes it can be packed in
one country, but come from another country. . . . Im thinking about rice and coffee. It might come from wherever.
Can be packed in Germany and come from inner Guinea, cant it? Nevertheless I know that we bought mostly Swedish
stuff. Thats what we do generally. [Were concerned] with meat. . . . What is it called? With cows and that.
It contaminates other products, but we try to find Swedish stuff.
(Sven referred here to the mad cow
disease.)
Britta had a strong opinion about buying apples
from far away:
Yes, it is madness to choose an
apple that has been freighted across half of the globe when it is in abundance in the gardens around. I dont
really understand why one should have the opportunity to choose, when it comes down to it.
They had not been able to implement the advice to
try to eat more legumes and tubers, mainly because the children did not like these vegetables. But happily Britta
admitted: We have bought organic oat flakes for the first time in our livesit was okay. Also coffee and
milk.
Table 2 summarises the food weights for 2 weeks
during the first registration in 1998 and during the 2 last weeks of the experiment in 2000. It was included in the
feedback report sent to the household about half a year after the experiment.
During the experiment, the Andersson household
cut down life cycle energy inputs by15%. The biggest changes during the experiment were the decrease in pork meat and
the increase in vegetables.
Table 2 Type of food consumed by the Andersson
household
Type of food
consumed during 2 weeks |
1998
|
2000
(Experiment)
|
Comments to the changes |
|
kg
|
kg
|
|
|
Beef |
3.8
|
4.1
|
More Swedish beef |
Bread |
4.8
|
5
|
Much bread from Sweden |
Cereals, grains |
4
|
3.1
|
Green-labelled porridge and oats, German
corn-flakes |
Drinks |
39
|
32
|
Eco-coffee during experiment, juice from
Denmark, use of water kettle often |
Egg
|
0.15
|
0.81
|
Eco-labelled
eggs
|
Fish
|
0.89
|
1.8
|
More fish, plaice
and cod from Sweden
|
Fowl
|
1.1
|
1.9
|
Swedish chicken
prepared in the oven
|
Fruit
|
2.8
|
1.8
|
Less fruit after,
Swedish marmalade and jam
|
Lamb
|
0
|
0
|
No change
|
Legumes
|
0
|
0
|
No change
|
Mixed dishes
|
6.8
|
7.3
|
Meat & potato
cuts and pancakes from Sweden
|
Pork
|
7.1
|
2.3
|
Less weight after,
Danish pork fillet, German sausages
|
Potatoes, pasta,
rice etc.
|
12
|
11
|
Eco-labelled pasta,
spaghetti from Italy
|
Sweet bread |
0.74
|
1,4
|
More cakes, bun and
gingerbread from Sweden |
Sweets |
1.7
|
0.94
|
Less sweets |
Vegetables
|
4.2
|
9
|
Big change, organic
carrots, many Swedish products
|
Yoghurt
|
3.8
|
3.8
|
Eco-labelled
yoghurt, Swedish products
|
|
Total |
Ca 90
|
Ca 90
|
|
|
|
|
|
Butter, margarine and sauces
not calculated |
One year after
The Andersson family, one year after the
experiment, was still very concerned about finding information on the origin of food products and tried to buy locally
produced food. They said this had become a routine but still found it difficult, and Sven thought that distribution
systems had developed in the wrong direction, making it more difficult for people to live up to their intentions. On
the whole, they did not believe that they had established any new habits but lived more or less the way they always
had. However, in the course of the interview several changes became evident.
For example, there is now a microwave oven in the
household, which they had resisted buying for a long time, having heard about a research project in which mice fed with
micro-waved food got cancer. The oven was a gift; it is now frequently used by the children for hot sandwiches. Another
obvious change was that the family had again started to separate organic waste. In the housing area they had moved
into, they felt there was a well-functioning municipal system for collection and treatment of organic waste. They were
nevertheless suspicious, especially Sven, about what happened to the organic waste after collection. They had heard
rumours that it was incinerated and not composted. Other changes that had taken place in the household were a reduction
in frozen food products, which they believed was a consequence of the experiment. They still, as they had before, tried
to avoid tinned food.
In analysing the checklists for frequency of
environmentally friendly everyday behaviours administrated in 1998 and 2001, it is clear that a general shift towards
higher frequencies had taken place. In 2001 the family always separated their waste into different types. In 1998 they
had not done so. In 2001 they often ate organic vegetables and meat and always organic eggs and cereals. In 1998 they
chose organic food products only sporadically.
Reaction to feedback
When Britta and Sven were asked about their
reaction to the report (with feedback on weights of food they consumed during the two registration periods and energy
input), they said they had just browsed through it but did not remember much. The researcher reminded them about the
content of the report, and they verified the accuracy. They were pleased and surprised that they had been able to cut
their energy use related to food consumed in the household by 15% during the experiment. They were aware of the big cut
in pork meat as well as a big increase in vegetables consumed. During the experiment they had also started to eat
home-made porridge instead of baked, ready-to-eat cerealsa habit they continued.
During the experiment the Anderssons bought many
eco-labeled food products. After a year they still did so and Britta said that they always bought organic coffee. Sven
was a bit doubtful but Britta persisted: But you have sort of accepted. You buy organic coffee yourself now in
fact even if you dont think so.
Following advice
There were also several pieces of advice in the
report on how to cut down life cycle food energy inputs, based on the data gathered from the household. Advice for the
Andersson family was to drink tap water instead of bottled juice; eat less cake; increase consumption of fruit from
Europe and Sweden; continue having porridge or müslie for breakfast instead of baked cereals; decrease oven
cooking of chicken; eat less cheese; and use vegetarian products such as banana, peanut-butter, or vegetables for open
sandwiches.
The Anderssons felt that they now ate more fruit
and vegetables then they had done during the experiment. They had found that the supply in the shops had increased and
was of better quality. The children had also started to eat vegetarian sandwiches. Another reason for their increase in
vegetable consumption was that they themselves started to grow vegetables, as commented by Britta:
We even eat tomatoes grown on free
land now when we grow it in our own gardennon-sprayed of course. And the children are eating more vegetables now.
It goes in periods up and down, but Frida eats a lot, more than you and me almost, on open sandwiches and
everything.
Sven nodded approvingly. But when it came to
cheese Britta admitted that they had more than ever.
The most important barriers, internal as well as
external, and opportunities for eco-sound food habits experienced by the Andersson household during the project period
are summarised in the figure below.
Figure 2 Barriers and opportunities for eco-sound food habits in the Andersson household
|
1998
|
2000
|
2001
|
Barriers
|
Different opinions
Distrust
High prices
Low awareness
|
Difficult to find information
Difficult to plan
|
Distrust
Distributions systems
High prices
Insufficient information on transports
|
Opportunities
|
Not
observed
|
Higher awareness
|
Higher awareness
More integrated family opinions
Own garden
Vegetable supply and quality
Waste collection
|
Mechanisms of change
Upon reflection about the food resource
management in the Andersson familys everyday life during the project implementation period, we conclude that they
changed their environmental behaviour to quite an extent. What is remarkable is that they themselves did not think they
had become more environmentally friendly but believed they lived more or less the way they always had. However, in the
course of the last interview they realised that they had gradually started to act more environmentally friendly and
even established some new routines. They had started to buy eco-labeled products to a great extent, had increased their
fruit and vegetable consumption, and ate more fish and less meat. They had started to drink tap water instead of
bottled water and to separate organic household waste. An established routine by the end of the project was to look for
the place of origin of foods.
Interacting with the environment
Several conditions in their living environment
had facilitated these changes. The supply of eco-labeled products had increased considerably over the three years
(Swedish Consumer Agency, 2000; KRAV, 2002). They also found that the supply and quality of fruit and vegetables had
improved. They now lived in an area with a collection system for organic waste, just as Sven requested at the beginning
of the project. They had moved to a house of their own where they could grow their own vegetables organically. In other
words, some external barriers had been removed (Gardner & Stern 1996). Several trends in the Swedish society also
supported green behaviour during these years, although environmental issues were no longer at the top of the agenda
(Bennulf, 1996). Such issues are nevertheless constantly present in the societal discourse, but now more taken for
granted. During the project implementation period, several events occurred that might have influenced the
Anderssons actions. The mad cow disease was much talked about in the media at the start of the project, in 1998,
to which Sven also referred. Even the Swedish prime minister expressed doubts about eating meat in an interview on
national TV (Aktuellt) (July 1, 2001) that created reactions from different groups. In 2001, there was the foot and
mouth disease outbreak in Great Britain. Besides, there was an ongoing debate about the conditions under which domestic
animals are bred, particularly pigs. All this might have given signals to the Andersson family to decrease their meat
consumption.
Feedback on household food
management
By participating in the project as a whole,
particularly in the experiment, the Anderssons became more conscious about the impact of food habits on the environment
and gained knowledge that led to new practices and changes in attitude. There is evidence that they learned in
action (Reason & Bradbury, 2001; Molander, 2000) and that there was some experience effect
(Ölander & Thøgersen, 1995). Although not much attention was paid to the feedback report when sent to
the household, it had some effect. The report illustrated how insignificant choices in their everyday life
made a difference. The reduction of life cycle energy input was made concrete to the family. They were satisfied that
they had achieved this reduction and gained confidence. It was important that the advice in the report was tailored to
the family itself, which could be seen in the behaviours that were enacted. The feedback might have been more effective
in a different form than a written report and if distributed soon after the experiment. Perhaps the feedback
alternatives the households had to choose between were too limited and should have included immediate and computer
aided information.
Changes in internal household
interaction
At the start of the project Sven voiced several
internal barriers to changing toward greener habits, such as distrust in the waste management system behaviour and in
eco-labeling. But, for the sake of peace in the house most of the time, he followed Brittas wishes. However,
toward the end of the project these internal barriers seemed to be slightly lower. By participating in the experiment
his attitudes towards green food habits may have become more positive. This observation supports the thesis that
attitudes and knowledge might change first after being involved in action (Ölander & Thøgersen, 1995).
Svens distrust of the municipal waste management system, however, was not without a real cause. Right at the
start of glass collection in the city, in the early 1990s, noncoloured and coloured glass were mixed for a short
period. Recently (2001) the municipality had problems with large scale composting during short periods, which resulted
in incinerating organic waste. To build up confidence among the public after such failures, which often get much
publicity in the media, is obviously very difficult.
During the last interview it appeared that the
gap in Britta and Svens attitudes toward environmental behaviour was smaller. An integration in their
relationship regarding these matters had taken place. Sven was buying more green-labeled products and was separating
organic waste. It was natural too for him to grow organically in their own garden. The change in his behaviour might be
explained by the theory of cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957, in Garder & Stern, 1996). The expectation from
society and from his wife to act environmentally friendly had, after a phase of hesitation and distrust, made him
finally change some of his everyday behaviour toward more environmentally friendly ones. However, Sven still did not
have full confidence in societal systems, such as food delivery and waste management systems.
Probably Britta and Sven now also felt they had
more time to act in environmentally friendly ways. The children were older; they were more established in their
professional roles and presumably had better incomes. They were doing well. They enjoyed family life and their new
house and had more abilities and opportunities to work for the environment (Ölander &Thøgersen, 1995).
Gradually their commitment and everyday activities had become more proenvironmental.
Other participating households
The other households participating in the study
were able to cut life cycle energy inputs in their diet during the experiment. Generally the trends in their food
consumption were similar to the pattern in the Andersson family. Among the changes of most importance for lowering
energy inputs were less meat, more vegetables and fruits and especially vegetables grown in the open, more fowl, eggs,
and fish but also an increased consumption of legumes. These households also frequently bought organic foodstuffs,
chose products from Sweden instead of imported ones and used electric kettles for boiling water for coffee and tea as
part of their efforts to achieve more energy efficient diets. The overall impression is that the households made
substantial efforts to abide by the recommendations given. Thus the findings indicate that, given information and
support, the households were able to experiment with new diets for environmental reasons. However, each household chose
its own strategies within the familys living pattern. Over time the pattern of behaviours was more varied. Some
households, like the Anderssons, kept up new eco-sound habits, while others fell back into old ones. Some even adopted
less eco-sound food habits.
Households and researchers in
partnership
Participatory action research methods are a way
to bring professionals and ordinary peoples practical needs and aspirations closer together for problem
identification and program design (Engberg, 2000). The project reported in this paper evolved, based on experiences and
data collected and analysed in proceeding phases. The interaction between researchers and households was intensive. In
total we visited the households seven times in their homes. It is remarkable that no household dropped out during the 3
years of the project. Four households moved out of the housing area but we were able to carry on the work with them. We
developed a good understanding of their needs and problems relevant to their situation and livelihood; this is what
Reason and Bradbury (2001) call living knowledge. We identified barriers and opportunities for changes
towards eco-sound food habits in the households. Learning in the action indeed took place, both for
researchers and households.
Engberg (2000),, and, among other professionals
working with families, have given the name reflective practitioner to this approach. It is indeed not
certain that the researcher who discovers a problem is the one who can solve it. The participation of the household in
our project in designing instruments and in the experiment is an important condition for identifying felt needs and
realistic solutions from the households' perspectives (Gardner & Stern, 1996). It is important to bridge gaps
between households and researchers in order to develop effective policies and consumer friendly production. There is
also a challenge to bridge the gap between scientific knowledge and everyday experience. The consequences of everyday
habits in terms of life cycle energy inputs over the life cycle were illustrated to the households. The project
provided an opportunity for the households to experience mediated scientific knowledge applied to the households
own everyday food habits.
The longitudinal and action research approach
made it possible to follow this emergent, evolutionary, and educational process engaging the Andersson family. The
results are specific to this household but yet generative and generalisable to other households. The positive change we
unraveled in the Andersson family taught us several things. To educate households to become green is a process that
takes time and must be supported by forces both from inside and outside the household. Being involved in an experiment
can have effects on everyday habits as well as feedback on resource use. Misconceptions and distrust are deeply rooted,
and complex information is difficult to convey to consumers. All this must be considered if we want to count on the
collaboration of households for ecologically sustainable development.
The human ecological approach helped us to focus
on the interaction between household members, as well as their interconnectiveness with their living environment. These
dynamic processes would not have been possible to capture by just studying individuals or individuals apart from their
household environment. The results reported in this paper add to the understanding of the functioning of the household
ecosystem, especially how change toward more ecologically sustainable food habits can occur. The findings show, indeed,
that there is a personal, social, and corporate responsibility for triggering such change. The personal is to implement
changes toward more environmentally friendly habits in everyday life. The social is to provide transparent and
user-friendly systems supporting households in their green efforts. The corporate is to change toward more
proenvironmental production and to provide relevant information about products and services in order to help households
make eco-sound decisions.
Notes
(1) Such changes include less meat,
cheese and farmed fish and fewer vegetables grown in greenhouses but more vegetables of the season, more locally
produced food and more fresh foods but less food with low nutritional value such as soft drinks and snacks
(Carlsson-Kanyama 1999; Kramer 2000; Sundquist & Jansson.; 2000; Jungbluth & Tietje (2000). An increase of
organically produced foods would also contribute to less environmental impact.
(2) The project is a joint venture between the
Department of Home Economics, Göteborg University and Environmental Strategies Research Group, Stockholm.
(3) Examples of what to do were: eat more fruit, vegetables,
tubers, and legumes; use a water kettle for heating water and a microwave oven for preparing food of 2-3 portions;
separate waste; and plan what to eat and shop in advance. Examples of what to avoid were: deep frozen and tinned food
and products that have been grown in green houses and/or which had been transported long distances. Also on the list
were: cut down consumption of rice, meat, and cheese; and avoid oven cooking for a small number of portions.
(4) All the names are fictitious and some
features of the household have been changed.
References
Åberg, H. (2000). Sustainable waste
management in the household - From international policy to everyday practice. Experiences from two Swedish field
studies. Göteborg, Sweden: Göteborg University.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980).
Understanding Attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, New York: Prentice Hall.
Alfredsson, E. (2002). Green consumption,
energy use and carbon dioxide emission. Unpublished PhD, Umeå, Sweden: Umeå University.
Becker, W. (1999). Svenskarna äter nyttigare
- allt fler väljer grönt [The Swedes are having healthier food - More and more people choose greens].
Vår Föda, 1, 24-27.
Bennulf, M. (1996). Miljöengagemanget i
graven [Death to environmental concern]?In S. Holmberg & L. Weibull (Eds.), Ett missnöjt folk [A
discontent people]? (Vol. 18). Göteborg, Sweden: SOM Institutet.
Bennulf M., & Gilljam, M. (1991). Snacka
går ju - men vem handlar miljövänligt [Once can always talk - but acting environmentally sound, who is
doing that]? In S. Holmberg (Ed.), Åsikter om massmdier och samhälle [Opinions on mass media and
society]. Göteborg, Sweden: Department of Political Science, Göteborg University.
Berge, B., & Ve, H. (2000). Action
research for gender equality. Buckingham, Philadelphia: Open University Press.
Biel, A. (1996). Väljer människor att
agera miljövänligt [Do people choose to act environmentally friendly]? In L. Lundgren (Ed.), Forskning om
livsstil och miljö. Fråga, forska och förändra [Research on life style and environment. Ask,
research and change]. Stockholm: Naturvårdsverket.
Biel, A., Larsson, M., & Gärling, T.
(1999). Köpbeteendets psykology - Miljömedvetenhet och vanor [The psychology of buying - Environmental
consciousness and habits]. In K. Ekström & H. Forsberg (Eds.), Den flerdimensionella konsumenten - en
antologi om svenska konsumenter [The multi-dimensional consumer - an anthology about Swedish consumers].
Göteborg, Sweden: Tre böker.
Bubolz, M., & Sontag, S. (1993). Human
ecology theory. In P. Boss, W. Doherty, R. LaRossa, W. Schumm, & S. K. Steinmetz (Eds.), Sourcebook of family
theories and methods: A contextual approach. New York: Plenum.
Carlsson-Kanyama, A. (1999). Consumption
patterns and climatic change. PhD thesis. Stockholm: Department of Systems Ecology, Stockholm University.
Carlsson-Kanyama, A., & Lindén, A.-L.
(2001). Trends in food production and consumption; Swedish experiences from environmental and cultural impacts.
International Journal of Sustainable Development, 4(4), 392-406.
Ekström, M. (1990). Kost, klass och
kön [Food, class and gender]. Unpublished PhD thesis. Umeå, Sweden: Department of Sociology, Umeå
University.
Ekström, M., Jonsson, L., Renström,
Å., & Shanahan, H. (1996). Matvanor och el i små lägenhetshushåll [Food habits and
electricity in small households living in apartments]. (Research Report 20). Göteborg, Sweden: Göteborg
University, Department of Home Economics and Crafts.
Ellegård, K., & Friberg, T. (1993).
Tiden bara riner förbi [Time just flies]. Stockholm: Swedish Consumer Agency.
Ellegård, K., & Wihlborg, E., (Eds).
(2001). Fånga vardagen. Ett tvärvetenskapligt perspektiv [Capture everyday life. An interdisciplinary
approach]. Lund, Sweden: Studentlitteratur.
Engberg, L. (2000). What next for CHEA and the
international development program? Canadian Home Economics Journal, 50(3), 129-131.
Gardner, G., & Stern, P. (1996).
Environmental problems and human behavior. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Harman, W. (1998) Global mind change. The
promise of the 21st century. San Francisco: Berrett-Koehler.
Hartman, A. (1979). Finding families: An
ecological approach to family assessment in adoption. Beverly Hills, California: Sage Publications.
Holm, L. (1991). Kostens forandring [The
change of diets]. Köpenhamn: Akademisk forlag.
Holman, A. (1983). Family assessment: Tools
for understanding and intervention. Beverly Hills, California: Sage Publications.
Jungbluth, N., & Tietje, O. (2000). Food
purchases: Impacts from consumers' point of view investigated with modular LCA. International Journal LCA, 5(3),
134-142.
KLSA. (in press). Närproducerad mat.
Miljövänlig, affärsmässig [Locally produced food. Environmentally friendly, commercially sound,
animal friendly]. Stockholm: KLSA.
Kramer, K. (2002). Food matters. On reducing
energy use and greenhouse gas emission from household food consumption. PhD thesis. The Netherlands: Centre of
Energy and Environment, University of Groningen.
KRAV. (2002). Information from web site home page
in March 2002, http://www.krav.se.
Ljungberg, C. (2001). Bra mat och dåliga
vanor. Om förtroendefulla relationer och oroliga konsumenter på livsmedelsmarknaden [Good food and bad
habits. About trustful relationships and worried consumers on the food market]. Lund, Sweden: Lund
University.
Lundgren, L. (2000) Knowing and doing. On
knowledge and action in environmental protection. Stockholm: Swedish Environmental Protection Agency.
Magnusson, M., Arvola, A., Koivsto Hursti, U.-K.,
Åberg, L., & Sjödén, P.-O. (2001). Attitudes towards organic foods among Swedish consumers.
British Food Journal, 103(3), 209-22.
Molander, B. (2000). Human beings and their
orientation in the world: On knowledge, understanding and information. In L. Lundgren (Ed.), Knowing and doing. On
knowledge and action in environmental protection. Stockholm: Swedish Environmental Protection Agency.
National TV. (Aktuellt), (July, 2001).
Nordic Council of Ministers. (2001).
Förbrugernas krav på födevarumärkning og vareinformation [Consumer demand for labeling of
foodstuff and other produce]. Tema Nord 2001-201. Copenhagen: Nordic Council of Ministers.
OECD. (2001). Household food consumption:
Trends, environmental impacts and policy responses (Sustainable Consumption: Sector Case Study Series JT00118490).
Paris: OECD. Working Party on National Environmental Policy.
Ölander, F., & Thøgersen, J.
(1995). Understanding of consumer behaviour as a prerequisite for environmental protection. Journal of Consumer
Policy, 18, 345-385.
Olsson, T., & Retzner, L. (1998).
Plockanalys av hushållens säck och kärlavfall. En studie i sex svenska kommuner [Analysis of
household waste bags and containers. A study of six Swedish Municipalities] (FoU 145). Malmö, Sweden:
Stiftelsen Reforsk.
Palojoki, P. (1997). The complexity of
food-related activities in a household context. Study of Finnish homemakers' food choices and nutrition knowledge.
Unpublished PhD. Helsingfors, Finland: Helsingfors University.
Paolucci, B., Hall, O. A., & Axinn, N.
(1977). Family decision making: An ecosystem approach. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Peterat, L. (1997). Linking the practices of home
economics and action research. Canadian Home Economics Journal, 47(3), 100-104.
Reason, P., & Bradbury, H. (Eds.). (2001).
Handbook of action research. Participative inquiry & practice. London: Sage.
Shanahan, H. (1997). Additional information to
households on waste weights: An experiment carried out in a community in Sweden where weight based billing had been
introduced. Paper presented at the IX International Conference on the Society for Human Ecology: Local and Global
Communities: Complexity and Responsibility. Bar Harbor, Maine.
Shanahan, H., Jonsson, L., & Renström,
Å. (1995). Matvanor och energianvändning i hushåll. En pilotstudie [Food habits and energy
consumption in households. A pilot study]. (Research Report 15). Göteborg, Sweden: Göteborg Universitet,
Department of Home Economics and Crafts.
Shanahan, H., & Säljö, R. (1993).
Purchases, food habits and waste management in households: Exploring responses to local composting. Journal of
Consumer Studies and Home Economics, 17, 197-205.
Shanahan, H., & Zetterstrand, H. (1992).
Inköp-kost-avfall. Ett hushållsekologiskt perspektiv på inköpens betydelse för avfallet
[Purchase-foods-waste. A household ecological approach to the importance of purchase behavior related to waste].
(Research Report 8). Göteborg, Sweden: Göteborg Universitet, Institutionen för slöjd och
hushållsvetenskap.
Sjödén, P.-0. (1994).
"Miljömedvetande" - en konsekvens av miljövänliga beteenden ["Environmental
consciousness" - A consequence of environmentally friendly behavior]? In L. J. Lundgren (Ed.), Livsstil och
miljö. Handlingsutrymme för förändring [Lifestyle and environment. Opportunities for change].
Stockholm: Forskningrådsnåmnden.
Solér, C. (1997). Att köpa
miljövänliga dagligvaror [A study of consumers' buying of ecologically friendly consumer goods].
Stockholm: Nerenius & Santérus.
Sontag, S. M., & Bubolz, M. M. (1996).
Families on small farms. East Lansing, Michigan: Michigan State University.
Sundquist, G. Å., & Jansson, A. -M.
(2000). Strength and limitation of localizing food production as a sustainable-building strategy: An analysis of bread
production on the island of Gotland, Sweden. Ecological Economics. Accepted for publication.
Sundquist, G. Å. (2000). The
environmental experts: On science's authority in environmental protection. Stockholm: Swedish Environmental
Protection Agency.
Swedish Consumer Agency (2000). Utbud och pris
på ekovaror. Rusultat från en undersökning av 56 ekologiska livsmedel på nio orter våren
2000 [Supply and price for eco-products. Results from an investigation of 56 organic foodstuffs in nine places in
Spring 2000]. Stockholm: Swedish Consumer Agency.
Swedish National Food Administration (2002).
http://www.slv.se. March 26, 2002.
The Swedish Government, Department of Civil
Affairs (1996). Konsumenterna och miljön [The consumers and the environment]. SOU 1996: 108.
Stockholm.
Thøgersen, J. (1998). Knowledge
barriers to sustainable consumption. Paper presented at the Nordic Consumer Research Conference, November 11-14,
Lillehammer, Norway.
Thøgersen, J. (2000). Promoting green
consumer behavior with eco-labels. Paper presented at the Workshop on Education, Information, and Voluntary
Measures in Environmental Protection, November 29-30, Washington, DC.
Tuomi-Gröhn, Y., & Palojoki, P. (2000).
Studying human action in the household. Canadian Home Economics Journal, 50(3), 113-120.
UNEP (2001). United Nations Environment
Program. http://www.unep.org/malmo-minesterial.htm. May 23, 2001.
Vaines, E. (1995). Family science and home
economics: Companion communities in search of meaning. Family Science Review, 8(1 & 2), 1-16.
|