Keywords: pre-bedtime routine, sleep characteristics, college students, professors
Abstract
Anecdotal evidence shows that activities in preparation for sleep affect certain characteristics
of sleep, such as duration and quality. This study tests this evidence. According to the literature, when
asking the question of whether pre-bedtime routine affects sleep characteristics, particularly of college students
and professors, it was hypothesized that a structured pre-bedtime routine positively affects sleep characteristics.
A web-based survey was distributed to college students and college professors to look into structure of pre-bedtime
routine and sleep quality. Two Pearson r correlations and two t-tests were used to analyze the data. After completion
of this study, there was no significance found between the variables of pre-bedtime routine and sleep characteristics.
Introduction
Given the rigors of college life, one would
suspect that sleep quality would be an issue. There has not been much research regarding the effects of pre-bedtime
routine on sleeping characteristics, especially on young and middle-aged adults. However, there has been a fair
amount of research relating to sleep and children as well as sleep and the elderly. Numerous factors relate to
sleep quality such as a structured routine, external stimuli, substances, cognitive strain, and daytime sleep.
Among the research studies conducted thus far, some factors seem to correlate and/or affect sleeping qualities
more than others, mainly structure of routine.
Sleep problems, such as trouble sleeping
or a lack of sleep, are of major concern for parents of young children. Approximately 20-30 percent of infants
and toddlers brought into pediatricians possibly suffer from a sleep problem. A 2008 study of 206 infants (ages
7–18 months) and 199 toddlers (18-36 months) tested the efficacy of pre-bedtime routine on sleep characteristics
(Mindell, Telofski, Wiegand, & Kurtz, 2009). The mothers of the children were instructed to follow a three-step
routine of preparing their child for bed including a bath, massage/lotion, and “quiet activities (e.g.,
cuddling, singing, lullaby) with lights out within 30 minutes of the end of the bath.” The study resulted
in 94 percent effectiveness with 80 percent significant improvement, subsequently leading to less waking, shorter
waking periods, increased sleep continuity, and decreased amount of time it took to fall asleep (Mindell, Telofski,
Wiegand, & Kurtz, 2009).
Another study by Chrsitodulu and Durand (2004)
involved four toddlers who all had developmental disabilities and sleeping difficulties. The researchers wanted
to minimize sleeping problems by implementing positive bedtime routines and restrictions on sleep. Positive bedtime
routines varied, but still had specific guidelines: each child’s parents constructed a routine following
those guidelines. Parents also restricted sleep time by setting bedtimes and wake-times for the children based
on their normal sleep duration. Similar to the Mindell et al. (2009) study, this approach reduced the number
of times the children awoke during the night. Beyond these findings, the Chrsitodulu and Durand study (2004)
was “successful in eliminating bedtime disturbances” as long as the routine continued for the duration
of the study. Research suggests that children’s sleep quality can be positively affected by routine.
Cultural differences have implications as well. For example, in a study comparing children in China and children
in the United States, Chinese children get one less hour of sleep than children in the United States. Chinese
children’s sleepiness is accounted for by less sleep, and in the United States’ children’s
sleepiness is accounted for by restless sleep and snoring (Liu, Liu, Owens, & Kaplan, 2005). Apart from
overseas cultural differences, subcultures within a society can have vast differences as well.
Although young children need healthy sleeping habits, the same is true for the elderly. A research article authored
by Lai and Good (2005) stated that half of the people they studied over the age of 65 had interrupted sleep or
difficulties getting a good night’s rest. Their study looked at the impact of listening to music while
falling asleep and during sleep. Lai and Good also sought to determine which factors influenced sleep quality,
such as caffeine ingestion, medication, etc. This particular study found that music was greatly beneficial in
many different areas related to sleep: quality, latency, efficiency, and daytime function (Lai & Good, 2005).
The middle-aged cohort is important to study as well. This cohort includes mostly people in the workforce, such
as doctors, factory workers, teachers, social workers, and even professors. It could be assumed that this age
group might include the idea of their having pre-bedtime routines habitualized, as well as mostly good sleeping
patterns. Research suggests this may or may not be the case. One study conducted by the National Sleep Foundation
said that 52 percent of women and 45 percent of men have problems sleeping/getting a good night’s rest
(“Why nearly half of all adults have trouble sleeping,” 1996). The same article went on to say that
stress was usually the main contributor to the problems middle-aged adults have with sleep. One of the conclusions
was that relaxing bedtime routines provide for more restful sleep.
Other research investigated pair sleep versus sleeping alone for middle-aged adults. The definition given of
pair sleep was “the practice of sleeping in couples in an individual bed with close contact” (Dittami,
Keckeis, Machatschke, Katina, Zeitlhofer, & Kloesch, 2007). Negative impacts of lack of sleep included effects
on cognition, ability to deal with stress, and immunity to diseases. The main purpose of this study was to determine
whether or not pair sleeping had more negative effects on people than sleeping alone. Conclusions drawn from
this study included the fact that women were more likely than men to be negatively influenced by pair sleep.
Interrupted sleep induced by another person was the main culprit.
College students are a culture all their
own. They have extremely high workloads, variable schedules, and a vast array of stimuli. In 1999, change in
sleeping habits was the highest stressor for college students at a particular university in the United States
(Ross, Nievling, & Heckert, 1999). In fact, sleep routine is one of the first habits to deviate according
to Jensen (2003). It is quite likely that this trend has continued over the years since the study was
conducted. A study about the effects of television on sleep-wake patterns for college students concluded that
restricted amounts of television viewing led to earlier bedtimes and longer sleeps periods. Managed television
viewing can also contribute positively by reinforcing “sleep-wake patterns” (Asaoka, Fukuda, Tsutsui, & Yamazaki,
2007).
The freedom of college students to make decisions
on their own concerning their well-being also may contribute to sleep patterns. Although it does not focus on
college students, a study concerning adolescents making their own decisions about sleep concluded that adolescents
who made their own bedtimes were more sleep deprived and had more sporadic bedtimes (Teufel, Brown, & Birch,
2007). Although this study researched adolescents, decision-making power over their own bedtimes pertains to
college students as well. Many college students are still biologically adolescents, and their physiology indeed
affects their sleep. Specifically, this age group has delayed sleep phases, meaning they naturally fall asleep
later than other age groups. According to the National Sleep Foundation, 59 percent of people from 18-19 years
of age labeled themselves as night owls (Gaultney, 2010). College students also report having unsatisfactory
sleep throughout the week and trying to make up for it on the weekends: 24 percent of college students claimed
to be dissatisfied in 1978, and 71 percent of students reported dissatisfaction in 2000 (Jensen, 2003). These
sleep patterns often have severe consequences on activities of daily living and their responsibilities (Gaultney,
2010).
Since pre-bedtime routines have the capability of influencing sleep, it is wise to consider what impacts the
quality of sleep. A study was completed on the impact of reading for pleasure as a pre-bedtime activity. In the
article, there were multiple reasons given for why individuals would enjoy reading for pleasure, especially pre-bedtime.
A group of 223 college students were the subjects for this particular study. The researcher found a significant
relationship between decreased sleep duration and increased amounts of pleasure reading (Kelly, 2009).
Another external stimulus that may have an impact on sleep is the presence of continuous white noise. Forquer
and Johnson (2007) were concerned about the influence of white noise on sleep characteristics. This study of
college students found that “white noise appears promising in reducing sleep problems in college students;
however, like medication disturbances may return when discontinued.”
Hypothesis 1 - Because all the cohorts discussed
previously seemed to respond well to routine and were affected similarly by certain stimuli, college students
will most likely experience comparable effects.
Hypothesis 2 - When asking the question of whether pre-bedtime routine affects sleep characteristics, particularly
of college students and professors, it is likely that pre-bedtime routine does indeed affect sleep characteristics.
Specifically, a structured routine will be a major positive contributor to such characteristics, especially in
the area of increased quality of sleep influenced by an increased steadiness of routine.
Method
Participants
This study surveyed the population of a midwestern
private Christian university. The whole student body was invited to participate, and the whole body of professors
was selected as a contrast group. There were 979 students (519 females and 460 males) and 105 professors in the
potential pool of respondents. Although most of the students’ ages ranged from 18 to 23, a handful of
them were older. The professors’ ages ranged from 30-70. The study gathered 213 respondents, with 17 being
professors and 189 being students.
Measures
The effects of pre-bedtime activities on
sleep characteristics were measured through a survey entitled the “Sleep and Pre-Bedtime Routine Survey” (Anderson,
Garner, & Manges, 2011) [see Appendix A]. The survey contained nineteen questions and consisted of a variety
of response-types, including multiple choice, yes/no, and matrix form. Demographic questions followed. The survey
was produced using SurveyMonkey for an online response.
The questions compared each participant’s
pre-bedtime activities and structure of their activities before bed to the qualities of sleep that they tended
to experience. The survey examined things such as television watching, showering, reading, video gaming, exercising,
and other activities that people may do before going to bed. It also examined items that participants might need,
or might need to avoid, in order to sleep. These included things such as a light, a television, a fan running,
and a noisemaker running. Each question was based on the participant’s average or normal tendencies.
Procedure
The survey was created through SurveyMonkey,
a resource specifically for making web surveys. With the permission of the university, the survey was e-mailed
to the participants using the university list serv for a mass e-mail. An administrator in charge of student body
and faculty e-mails sent the e-mail per request of the researchers. Through the e-mail, the participants could
click on a hyperlink in order to take the survey. When the participants finished the survey, they could click “Done,” and
the results would be sent to a SurveyMonkey account for further analysis.
Results
The data were analyzed using a Pearson r correlation coefficient in order to test the possible correlation between
pre-bedtime routine and quality. In order to test whether a correlation existed between pre-bedtime routine and
sleep quality, the researchers constructed quality and structure indexes using SPSS. All of the pre-bedtime routine
question results were placed into an index labeled “Structure Index” and all of the results of the
quality of sleep questions were placed into an index labeled “Quality Index.” The answers were quantified
pertaining to structure and quality and ranges were created to indicate scores on the indexes. Then, using the
Pearson r correlation coefficient, the two indexes were compared. No significant correlation was found between
pre-bedtime routine and sleep characteristics (r = .013) (see Table 1). The results
failed to reject the null hypothesis.
Table 1.
In order to examine individual aspects, participants’ rigidity
of routine were examined and compared with how rested they felt after waking. This accounted for a relationship
not between structure of routine on sleep quality but on perceived quality—how the participant felt after
waking rather than his or her actual efficacy of sleep. According to the literature, one would have expected
that the rigidity of the pre-bedtime routine, above all other variables, would have contributed to more perceived
restfulness. However, the findings from this study were not so decisive. No significant correlation was found
between a structured routine and restfulness (r = -.015) (see Table 2).
Table 2.
This study examined professors contrasted with college students, particularly testing whether the two cohorts
differed in the rigidity of the structure of their pre-bedtime routines and in the quality of their sleep. Two
t-tests were run to compare these groups. There was no significant difference found between college students
and professors in the area of structure of pre-bedtime routine (t = .619) (see Table 3) and
no significant difference again for their quality of sleep (t = .450) (Table 4).
Table 3.
*For the Group Statistics Table, 1 = College Student and 2 = Professor
Table 4.
*For the Group Statistics Table, 1 = College Student and 2 = Professor
Discussion
After completing our review of the literature, we hypothesized that a structured pre-bedtime routine would produce
better sleep quality shown through different characteristics. Now as we look back at our literature review, we
found that a structured routine can (but not always) affect sleep quality in a positive manner. As supported
by the research conducted prior to this collection of data, sleep routine of college students is one of the first
habits to deviate from the norm (Jensen, 2003). Another factor contributing to pre-bedtime routine is the newfound
freedom that college brings to students. Because of that freedom, college students may be more likely to not
have strict routines.
Upon completion of the analysis of data, we determined that a structured pre-bedtime routine has little impact
on sleeping characteristics and/or quality. This finding did not support our hypothesis, because we thought that
a more structured routine would lead to better sleep overall. The results from this study are important because
the findings dissent from a common sense point of view.
There were also several limitations of our research. The survey distributed to the participants was probably
the most problematic in that there were several ways in which some of the questions could have been interpreted,
e.g., the meaning of whether the person would sleep poorly if they did not follow it or how strictly they actually
follow their routine (which question three was attempting to answer; see Appendix A). Better descriptive words
might have been useful, and there may have been problems of misinterpretation. In order to fix this, we could
separate it into two different questions. Question eleven created an issue for participants because it did not
address what we were looking for—how often a person is tired enough that they want to sleep throughout
the day. Similarly, number twelve could have had better answer options. One important limitation that we considered
at the beginning of this project was the population sample. College students are not representative of the general
population, especially because their sleep schedules vary so much, but the research was intended to examine sleep
habits of college students.
There was a very strong correlation between health-related characteristics, such as getting more sleep leads
to feeling less tired throughout the day and structured routine (especially for bedtime). For future research,
we would suggest that sleep habits and characteristics be studied in light of health and well-being. By doing
so, future researchers may learn more about the impact of structured sleeping habits on overall daily functioning.
When considering the growing academic population, sleep is an important area of research. Research projects in
the future should compare the academic population with other groups of people in the general population.
References
Asaoka, S., Fukuda, K., Tsutsui, Y., & Yamazaki, K. (2007). Does television viewing cause delayed and/or
irregular sleep-wake patterns? Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 5, 23-27.
Christodulu, K. V., & Durand, M. (2004). Reducing bedtime disturbance and night waking using positive bedtime
routines and sleep restriction. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 130-139.
Dittami, J., Keckeis, M., Machatschke, I., Katina, S., Zeitlhofer, J., & Kloesch, G. (2007). Sex differences
in the reactions to sleeping in pairs versus sleeping alone in humans. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 5,
271-276.
Forquer, L., & Johnson, M. (2007). Continuous white noise to reduce sleep latency and night waking in college
students. Sleep and Hypnosis, 9, 60-66.
Gaultney, J. F. (2010). The prevalence of sleep disorders in college students: Impact on academic performance. Journal
of American College Health, 59, 91-97.
Jensen, D. R. (2003). Understanding sleep disorders in a college student population. Journal of College
Counseling, 6, 25-34.
Kelly, W. E. (2009). To wake, to wake, perchance to read: Sleep duration and reading for pleasure. Reading
Improvement, 46, 221-226.
Lai, H. L., & Good, M. (2005). Music improves sleep quality in older adults. Journal of Advanced Nursing,
49, 234-244.
Liu, X., Liu, L., Owens, J. A., & Kaplan, D. L. (2005). Sleep patterns and sleep problems among schoolchildren
in the United States and China. Pediatrics, 115, 241-249.
Mindell, J. A., Telofski, L. S., Wiegand, B., & Kurtz, E. S. (2009). A nightly bedtime routine: Impact on
sleep in young children and maternal mood. Sleep, 32, 599-606.
Ross, S. E., Niebling, B. C., & Heckert, T. M. (1999). Sources of stress among college students. College
Student Journal, 33, 312-317.
Teufel, J. A., Brown, S. L., & Birch, D. A. (2007). Sleep among early adolescent students. American
Journal of Health Studies, 22, 10-17.
Why nearly half of all adults have trouble sleeping. (1996). Jet, 89, 54-58.
Appendix A
Sleep and Pre-Bedtime Routine Survey
- Check all of the following activities that you participate in as part of your before-bed activities. (Check
all that apply.)
____ Homework
____ Watching television or movies
____ Reading for pleasure
____ Playing video/computer games
____ Exercise
____ Snacking
____ Journaling
____ Brushing teeth
____ Showering
____ No specific activity
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
- How important is it to you to follow that routine each and every night?
____ Absolutely necessary
____ Somewhat necessary
____ Neutral
____ Somewhat unnecessary
____ Not necessary at all
____ Unsure
- In general, does your pre-bedtime routine vary …?
____ Daily
____ Occasionally
____ Rarely
____ Never
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
- Does your pre-bedtime routine change depending on where you sleep? (I.e., home, dorm, at a friend’s
house, etc.?)
____ Yes
____ No
____ Does not apply
- How often do you tend to wake up during the night?
____ Usually not at all
____ Usually once or twice
____ Usually more than twice
- If you were trying to sleep, would any of the following distract you? (Check all that apply.)
____ People
____ Music
____ Television/movies
____ Light
____ Fans
____ Thoughts
____ Nothing distracts me
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
- Check any and all of the following that you find necessary in order to sleep, and rate how necessary they
are to you. (Matrix here in actual survey.)
*Matrix top options include: absolutely necessary, somewhat necessary,
neutral, somewhat unnecessary, not necessary at all
*Matrix side answers include:
____ Background noise (fan for sound included)
____ Spouse
____ Roommate
____ Sibling
____ Fan for temperature
____ Light source
____ Total darkness
____ Other (please specify and rate): __________________________________
- How many hours of sleep do you tend to
average per night? (This does not include naps.)
*A drop-down box with answers ranging from 0 to 12+
- When you wake up, how rested do you feel?
____ Well rested
____ Somewhat rested
____ Neutral
____ Somewhat unrested
____ Very unrested
- Do you toss and turn while sleeping?
____ Yes
____ No
____ Unsure
- How frequently do you feel the need to nap?
____ Daily
____ Occasionally
____ Rarely
____ Never
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
- How frequently do you actually nap?
____ Daily
____ Occasionally
____ Rarely
____ Never
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
- Do you feel that you function well after an
average night of sleep?
____ Yes
____ No
____ Unsure
- Do you take over-the-counter or prescription medication(s) for sleep?
____ Yes
____ No
- Do you have any sleep disorders? If so, please list.
____ Unsure
____ No
____ Yes (please specify): _______________________________
- What is your gender?
____ Male
____ Female
- What is your age? (Please write in a number.) _________
- Are you a college student or a professor?
____ College Student
____ Professor (*skip logic was used so that professors were finished with the survey)
- What is your classification as a student (by credit)?
____ Freshman
____ Sophomore
____ Junior
____ Senior
____ Other (please specify): __________________________________
Thank you for participating in our survey!! If you have any questions or concerns please e-mail us.