Abstract
The
goal of this study was to explore the relationship between age and self-esteem
in female children and adolescents. Females in grades 2-12 in the
Camden-Frontier School District were examined regarding their self-esteem. The
junior high and high school participants completed the Rosenberg Self-esteem
Scale to assess levels of self-esteem. The elementary school participants
completed a version of the same scale that was slightly modified for age. The
age related changes in self-esteem were determined using an ANOVA. It was
predicted that self-esteem would begin at high levels in early childhood, drop
around junior high, and increase again in high school, resulting in a
curvilinear relationship.
Self-esteem in Females
throughout Childhood and Adolescence
�Self-esteem
is a widely studied phenomenon that has gained much attention in our culture.
Self-esteem as defined by Aronson, Wilson, and Akert (2005) is the extent to
which people view themselves as good, competent, and decent. It is a complex
concept that has multiple variables, each of which can serve to enhance or
hinder an individual�s concept of him/her self. Self-esteem is influenced by
many factors. This study is mainly focusing on age and gender. However, in
order to acquire a complete picture of self-esteem it is imperative to explore
beyond the variables of age and gender.
Age
Research on age and self-esteem has mostly focused
on children�s transition into adolescence. The focus on this age range is not
surprising considering the transition into adolescence has long been considered
an important one. Erik Erikson�s stages of psychosocial development describe
the adolescent years as a time of either establishing identity or experiencing
role or identity confusion. It would logically follow that this new formation
of identity, or lack thereof, would play a significant role in terms of
developing self-esteem. The general trend found in the research suggests that
the transition into adolescence results in a decline in self-esteem (Burnett,
1996; Cantin & Boivin, 2004; Eccles, Wigfield, Flanagan, Miller, Reuman,
& Yee, 1989; Robins, Trzesniewski, Tracy, Gosling, & Potter, 2002;
Twenge & Campbell, 2001). More specifically, one longitudinal study
conducted in four-waves followed children from sixth to seventh grade measuring
them once in the fall and once in the spring of each year. The resulting data indicated
that self-esteem was lowest in the fall of the seventh grade year, right after
the transition to junior high had been made (Eccles et al., 1989). Another
study by Burnett (1996), which also found a decline in self-esteem in
adolescence, suggested that the decline may result from the changes in thinking
that occur during adolescence that allow the individuals to see themselves less
egocentrically and, thus, more accurately.�
However, it has been demonstrated that this general
drop in self-esteem during adolescence is not permanent. Twenge and Campbell
(2001) found that scores for both males and females began to progressively rise
after junior high, although the males� self-esteem began to increase more in
high school whereas the females� rose more significantly at the college age.
Likewise, Frost and McKelvie (2004) found lower levels of self-esteem in high
school students when compared to elementary and university students. The lower
levels of self-esteem recorded in the high school period of this research
supports the idea that there is a decline during adolescence as well as the
idea that there is a subsequent increase after adolescence.
Furthermore, in order to address the absence of
life-span self-esteem research, Robins et al., (2002) conducted a study that
included participants ranging from age 9 to age 90. They found the highest
levels of self-esteem in children ages 9 to 12, and, in agreement with the
previous research, they found a sharp decline in self-esteem during
adolescence. However, unlike some of the former research, their data suggested
that the decline that began in adolescence continued through the college years
as well. The trend in self-esteem, as they observed it, rose instead during the
post-college age, 23- to 29-years-old, and then stabilized in the 30s. The
following decades, the 40s up to the 60s, were, like the post-college years,
marked by another gradual increase. This increase, however, appeared to reverse
once reaching the 60s where self-esteem began an unrelenting downward trend.
They observed that the decline was steeper between the 70s and 80s than between
the 60s and 70s and that the levels of self-esteem in the 80s were as low as
the levels observed in adolescence. Overall, self-esteem appears to be high in
early childhood, decrease during adolescence, rise gradually throughout
adulthood, and decrease in old age.
Weight and Gender
One variable that is often related to self-esteem is
one�s body weight, specifically being overweight. A plethora of literature is
available on body weight and obesity, especially considering the attention it
has been given in recent years. In modern times, �attention has especially
focused on how having a body weight that deviates from normative proscriptions
may affect the self, especially the way in which people evaluate themselves�
(Miller & Downey, 1999, p. 68).
Because self-esteem is partly a reflection of
others� appraisals and others tend to view the obese negatively, overweight
people may consequently devalue themselves (Miller & Downey, 1999).
Likewise, obese people may devalue themselves because �they fall short of
internalized social standards for acceptable body weight� (p. 69). A truly
obese person is more likely to be devalued by society and �even if the person
does not attribute this devaluation to weight, he or she may be aware of the
devaluation and consequently suffer low self-esteem� (p. 70). In essence, obese
people have a higher tendency to dislike themselves for being above a
culturally accepted weight. Physical appearance is the most visible aspect of
the self, and in modern Western society being overweight labels the individual
with a societal identity that is not highly valued by others (Miller &
Downey, 1999). These negative effects on the self-esteem of an individual are
especially prevalent in females. Moreover, children and adolescents, middle
class and upper class, and White Americans also feel the negative effects of
self-esteem more than others (Gusella, Clark, & Van Roosmalen, 2004;
Kjaerbye-Thygesen, Munk, Ottesen, & Kruger 2004; Kostanski, 2004; Kostanski
& Gullone, 2004). The rationale for these differences is that �there are
gender, age, socioeconomic status, and ethnic differences in standards for
thinness, cultural value placed on being thin, and the prevalence or normalcy
of obesity within these groups (Miller & Downey, 1999, p. 71).
Gender is the variable that is most commonly assumed
to predict how weight affects self-esteem. Females have been examined
extensively in relation to weight and body dissatisfaction throughout history
and especially in recent years. Boys have been increasingly shown to have
significantly higher self-esteem than girls (Davies & Brember, 1999).
Persistently, females show a significant increase in body dissatisfaction,
reflecting a predominant desire to be thinner (Al-Sendi, Bahrain, &
Musaiger, 2004; Gusella et al., 2004; Kjaerbye-Thygesen et al., 2004;
Kostanski, 2004; Kostanski & Gullone, 2004; Miller & Downey, 1999).
These women and girls are also likely to perceive themselves as obese or
overweight and express dissatisfaction with their body even though they are not
technically obese (Miller & Downey, 1999). This perceived obesity has been
shown to have a stronger relationship in individuals with low self-esteem than
those who are actually obese and overweight.
Females� beliefs that they are too heavy begin at an
early age; as early as age 5, girls are affected by how others perceive them
(Kjaerbye-Thygesen et al., 2004; Miller & Downey, 1999; Smith, 2001). It
has also been suggested that severe life events occurring in childhood or
adolescence may increase the risk for a female to consider herself too heavy
(Kjaerbye-Thygesen et al., 2004). Likewise, beginning to drink at an early age
as well as experiencing first intercourse at an early age both increase the
�risk of subsequent body dissatisfaction� (p. 283). Also, according to
Kjaerbye-Thygesen et al. (2004), this increase in body dissatisfaction may be
explained by an early start of dangerous behaviors being attributed to a personality
with a higher susceptibility to low self-esteem. For example, �severe childhood
life events, high vulnerability, and low self-esteem are associated with the
development of eating disorders� (p. 283). Likewise, �women are far more likely
than men to seek psychotherapy and other interventions to control their body
weight or to cope with adverse psychological effects of being heavier than they
think they should be� (Miller & Downey, 1999, p. 71). Although research
indicates that females have the most body dissatisfaction, males have also been
observed as having some body dissatisfaction. Kostanski (2004) suggested that
being overweight was considered unacceptable for males as well as females.
However, for boys, dissatisfaction with being too thin was also apparent.
Clearly, perceived obesity or true obesity seems to be a factor in low
self-esteem for many females and some males.
Markedly, self-esteem declines and body
dissatisfaction rises from childhood to adolescence (Al-Sendi et al., 2004;
Kostanski, 2004; Kostanski et al., 2004; Miller & Downey, 1999). �Although
dissatisfaction was present in both adolescents and children, it increased
significantly with age for males and females� (Kostanski, 2004, p. 5). One
suggestion for this difference is that people tend to gain weight as they age.
Miller and Downey (1999) suggest that being overweight is:
. . . more deviant and therefore more stigmatizing
for children and adolescents than for adults. Furthermore, appearances may be
increasingly important during adolescence and early adulthood, when people are
preoccupied with dating and mating. Therefore, young people, especially
adolescents, should show a stronger relation between being overweight and
self-esteem than adults. (p. 71)
According to Direnfeld (2003), weight and body image
play an important role in determining a child�s sense of self and weight
problems are often linked with issues of low self-esteem. Likewise, positive
correlations between self-esteem and attainment, which is defined as the successful
functioning of the individual, have been reported. It is also recognized that
self-esteem can be affected by how an individual is viewed by peers, teachers,
and parents (Davies & Brember, 1999). Children with healthy self-esteem
generally feel good about themselves and tend to be kind and considerate
(Direnfeld, 2003). These children also have an understanding of how their
behavior affects others and have a view of themselves that says that they are
at least equivalent to their peers or are content with any difference.
Self-esteem is relational. With self-esteem, the child not only feels good
about himself or herself independently but also in relation to others.
Sexuality
Research on the relationship between sexuality and
self-esteem has produced mixed results when comparing males and females. A
longitudinal study by Spencer, Zimet, Aalsma, and Orr (2002) found that males
who have high self-esteem are more
than twice as likely to become sexually active earlier than their peers.
Conversely, they found that females who have low self-esteem are three times more likely to become sexually
active earlier than their peers. The difference, they theorize, may come from
the contrasting views regarding sexuality held by the different genders.
Females may view sex as a way to achieve maturity, intimacy, acceptance, or
feelings of self-worth. Subsequently, sex becomes a vehicle for girls with low
self-esteem to attempt to achieve better feelings about themselves. Males, on
the other hand, may be inclined to see sex as an accomplishment, a �badge of
honor that is celebrated within the peer culture� (p. 583). This attitude could
serve as a catalyst for those boys who are already confident in themselves to
seek out sex.�
However, these results are not undisputed. A study
by Longmore, Manning, Giordano, and Rudolph (2004) found that boys who
exhibited levels of high self-esteem were more likely to initiate intercourse
at later ages. Furthermore, some studies have suggested that, although
self-esteem may be a factor in the age of onset of sex, it does not always
appear to be the primary one. Longmore et al. (2004) suggested that depressive
symptoms have a greater impact than self-esteem when predicting initiation of
sexual behavior in adolescents. Also, Slicker, Patton, and Fuller (2004) found
self-esteem to have only an indirect effect on adolescents engaging in early
sexual behavior.�
School, Peers, & Parents
School, peers, and parents have been found to
influence and affect a child�s self-esteem. G�din and Hammarstr�m (2003) looked
at whether or not the changes in one�s psychosocial school environment would
influence one�s health development. Psychosocial school environment involves
the social and the psychological aspects of one�s environment. The study was
done over a 3-year period. The results showed that girls in the older group
conveyed negative health development with a decreased sense of worth along with
increased somatic and psychological symptoms. They came to the conclusion that
�the negative development in pupils� health and self-worth could partly be
explained by the more unfavorable psychosocial environment that prevails at
school at the senior level� (p. 169).
A study
carried out by King, Vidourek, Davis, and McClellan (2002) investigated how
self-esteem was affected through school connectedness among fourth graders.
This was completed through a four-dimension mentoring program. The results of
this study concluded that school based programs, such as mentoring, can improve
a student�s self-esteem through focusing on different aspects in a child�s life
such as academic success and school, peer, and family connectedness.
Significant increases were found in each area of focus.
A study examining how peers affect each others�
self-worth discovered that adolescents with a low sense of worth reported
higher levels of jealousy among friendships (Parker, Low, Walker, & Gamm,
2005). Parents have also been found to affect their children�s self-esteem.
Scheffler and Naus (1999) observed that as the amount of affirmation from the
fathers towards their daughters increased, the daughters� self-esteem
subsequently increased.� Another study
exploring father-daughter relationships found the more overprotective a father
was of his daughter, the lower her self-esteem (Mori, 1999).
Laible, Carlo, and Roesch (2004) examined the direct
and indirect effects that peers and parents have on self-esteem. They believed
that there were mediating factors, such as empathy, prosocial behavior, and
aggression that would have an effect on self-esteem. One mediating factor that
produced significant results was prosocial behavior, which consists of actions
done with the purpose of benefiting others. Specifically, they found that
�parent attachment and prosocial behavior were significantly and positively
related to self-esteem�(p. 710).
Socioeconomic Status
Several studies have reported significant findings
regarding the effects of socioeconomic status (SES) on self-esteem. Generally,
research suggests that individuals with a higher SES have higher levels of
self-esteem than individuals who have a lower SES (Baharudin & Luster,
1998; Hare, 1977; Robins et al., 2002). Some studies have focused on the impact
of SES for all age groups, such as Robins et al. (2002).� Others have focused more specifically on the
consequences for children who live in families with a low SES. Christoffersen
(1994) found that parental unemployment contributes to the depletion of
children�s self-esteem. Whitbeck, Simons, Conger, Lorenz, Huck, and Elder
(1991) suggest that low SES has an indirect effect on the self-esteem of
adolescents because it causes a decrease in the support and involvement of the
parents. They do not think that it is necessarily the family�s economic
condition in and of itself that affects a child�s self-esteem, but that it is
how the economic status affects the parental-child relationship that makes the
difference.
Similarly, a study on the relationship between
mothers and their 6- to 8-year-old children found that mothers who had higher
levels of education and higher levels of family income provided higher quality
home environments for their children. Furthermore, these higher quality
environments were found to be related to their children�s levels of achievement
(Baharudin & Luster, 1998). In addition, Lent and Figueira-McDonough (2002)
found that as long as children with a low SES feel that they have competence in
one major area, either home or school, they are able to maintain a high sense
of global self-worth. It is when the low SES children have low levels of
competence in both major areas that they have significantly lower ratings of
global self-worth. Whether direct or indirect, SES does appear to have some
impact upon self-esteem.
Media/Pop Culture
Multiple studies have determined that media has an
impact on the self-esteem of children and adolescents. Specifically, it has
been observed that girls who view commercials of appearance report higher body
dissatisfaction than when viewing nonappearance commercials (Hargreaves &
Tiggemann, 2003). Johansson, Lundh, & Andersson (2005) focused on viewing
commercials of appearance. They found that adolescent girls had increased body
dissatisfaction while boys, on the contrary, were not affected by the
commercials.� Commercials of appearance
are those in which portray the ideal female image and products to improve
appearance. Commercials of nonappearance are those that promote a product such
as food. It was found in a study that body satisfied women responded positively
to thin ideal images.
Another image that is popular in the media and pop
culture is Barbie. Kuther and McDonald (2004) researched children�s and
adolescents� experiences with, and views of Barbie. Through this research they
found adolescent boys and girls believe Barbie has an effect on girls� self-concept.
They also believed Barbie to be a poor role model. The females in the research
study perceived the Barbie doll toys as �a dangerous influence on girls�
perceptions of beauty, body image, and self-concept� (p.48). With this research
in mind, one can see that media can have an influence on the self-esteem of an
adolescent.��
Hypothesis
According to the literature it is clear that there are many factors
that contribute to one�s self-esteem. Specifically, the relationships between
self-esteem and age, weight, gender, sexuality, school, peers, parents,
socioeconomic status, and media have been considered. One subset of the
research that is incomplete is the dimension of age. In fact, Demo (1992)
suggests that the research is �lopsided, with 12- and 13-year-olds forming the
floor and 18- to 22-year-olds representing the ceiling of our convenience
samples� (p. 323). Although more research has been conducted since Demo�s
study, there is still unevenness amongst the age groups. Young children and
older adults are often left out. Furthermore, the research suggests that
self-esteem drops in adolescence, but it is not clear as to the trend that
self-esteem has prior to that age. In order to address some of these issues,
this study examined self-esteem from childhood through adolescence. Also,
because the evidence strongly suggests that self-esteem is not often as great
an issue for males, this specific study focused mainly on females. For these
reasons, the proposed hypothesis was that self-esteem in females will begin at
high levels in early childhood, drop around junior high, and increase again in
high school, resulting in a curvilinear relationship.
Methods
Participants
A total of 156 female students from the
Camden-Frontier School District were used as participants in this study.� Informed Consent forms were distributed to
the participant�s parents prior to the study. Participants consisted of grades
2-12.
Measures
Self-esteem is defined as �a positive or negative orientation toward
oneself; an overall evaluation of one�s worth or value� (University of
Maryland, 2004).� It was measured in
this study using Rosenberg�s (1965) Self-Esteem Scale (RSE). The RSE consists
of ten questions that address one�s feelings about him/herself.� The Rosenberg scale takes approximately ten
minutes to complete. The ten-question Likert scale asks the participant to
respond with one of four choices, ranging from �strongly agree� to �strongly
disagree.� Scores range from 10 to 40. A high score on this survey indicates
that the participant feels that he or she is a person of value and is worthy of
self-respect. A low score on this scale indicates the feelings of
self-rejection, self-dissatisfaction, and self-contempt (James, n.d.).
Rosenberg did not indicate discrete cutoff points to distinguish high and low
self-esteem. The researchers considered scores of 15-25 to be normal.�
This scale is known to generally have high reliability and test-retest
correlation in the range of .82 to.88, and Cronbach�s alpha for various samples
are in the range of .77 to .88 (Owens, 2001;University of Maryland, 2004; Wells
& Marwell, 1976). Rosenberg originally developed this scale to measure
self-esteem in adolescents. The middle school and high school participants used
the original measure while the elementary participants completed the
�elementary school version.� Some words from the original scale were
substituted with age appropriate words. It was also necessary for some wording
to be supplemented in order for the young children to comprehend the statements.
For example, instead of �I feel that I�m a person of worth, at least on an
equal plane with others,� statements like �I feel as important as others� were
proposed for the second and fifth grade participants (See Appendices A and B to
compare measures).
Procedure
Our procedures for the elementary school differed
from that of the procedure for the junior high and the high school. Elementary
school students were given an informed consent form to have their guardians
return prior to the study. Likewise, the middle school and high school
participants were also given informed consent forms to be completed by their
guardians. For this research, elementary school students received the
�elementary school version� of the RSE. All students remained in their assigned
seats. Boys and girls were both asked to complete the RSE. The measure was read
to the elementary student participants. Additionally, they were asked to write
a �B� or a �G� at the top of the paper in order to indicate their gender.
During data analysis, only the girls� results were analyzed in regards to the
initial hypothesis.
The junior high and high school students received
the original version of the RSE. Again, males and females both completed the
measure and indicated their gender. In all classrooms the researchers followed
a script when distributing the measure and had a script for anticipated
questions (See Appendices D and E for scripts). Questions that came up, which
were not anticipated, were written down, along with the answer that was given,
so that the same answer would be given should the question be asked again. Any
students that did not return the informed consent form were asked to remain in
their seats. Furthermore, they were instructed that they may read or do
homework quietly for the short duration of the survey.
Data Analysis
To measure the variance between groups an ANOVA was
used. In addition, the variability within each age category was evaluated using
each group�s standard deviations.
Results
The number of surveys completed was 156 which ranged
from second to twelfth grade. Of these 156 surveys, 14 were from Grade 2, 18
from Grade 3, 6 from Grade 4, 25 from Grade 5, 10 from Grade 6, nine from Grade
7, 14 from Grade 8, 17 from Grade 9, 20 from Grade 10, nine from Grade 11, and 11
from Grade 12. To obtain the results from the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale an
ANOVA was used. The hypothesis that self-esteem in females would change between
age groups was not supported. The results showed no significant difference
between the grade levels, F (10, 155) = 1.551; p = .127 (see Figure 1).
Figure 1
A further analysis examining gender and esteem
scores was conducted. All male scores were analyzed by grade level. They too
showed no significant difference, F (10, 142); = .749; p = .677(see Figure 2).
Additionally, male and female scores for each grade were combined to find an
average score. These average scores for each grade level were compared
revealing no significant difference, F (10, 299) = .500; p = .889 (see Figure
3). The male and female scores were compared also indicating no significant
difference between the two genders, F (1, 299) = .507; p = .477 (see Figure 4).
Figure 2
Figure 3
Figure 4
Discussion
The primary goal of this
study was to examine changes in female�s self-esteem throughout childhood and
adolescence. It was predicted that self-esteem would follow a curvilinear
pattern: beginning high in early childhood, dropping in early adolescence, and
rising again in later adolescence. Results of this study produced no significant
differences in self-esteem between the age groups. The results were contrary to
the majority of the findings discussed previously.�
Although it was hypothesized
that there would be significant changes in levels of self-esteem between the
grades, the average scores for all grades fell within the normal levels of
self-esteem. This finding was consistent when examining female�s scores, males�
scores, and the combined scores of males and females within each grade. Within
each individual grade there was some fluctuation between scores, however most
scores varied within the normal range. In addition, our further research
involving the scores of the male participants produced another finding that was
contradictory to the previously examined research (Davies & Brember, 1999).
Although the difference was not statistically significant, males, on average,
reported lower self-esteem than females.
There were a number of
variables that could have impacted the results producing findings inconsistent
with the previous research on self-esteem. One explanation for the results is
based upon the structure of the school that was studied. The Camden-Frontier
School District consists of one building, which contains all grade levels from
kindergarten through 12th grade. Due to the unique set up of this
school system, students never transfer to a new building. This results in
students spending their entire educational career in the same building with the
same teachers, staff, and students. Many previous studies focused on schools
where transition occurs between elementary school and middle school and again
between middle school and high school. It is postulated that this absence of
experiencing entering a new school with new students may, in part, account for
the fairly homogenous esteem scores across the grade levels.
Furthermore, the sizes of
the grade levels and classes in the Camden-Frontier School District are also
important to consider. Each grade level consists of between 40 and 65 students,
and each class consists of between 18 and 30 students. The small number of
students in each grade, along with the small overall population of the school,
may result in homogeneity among the students. Most students are acquainted with
the students near their age as well as the other student�s families. It is
often quoted in the community that �everybody knows everybody and his/her
business.� This familiarity may result in a level of comfort that does not
bring about as much competition or concern about image as what may occur in
larger schools.�
There were a number of
variables that could have potentially influenced the validity of the study.
Because of a limited time frame, students could only be surveyed between the
hours of eight and three. Furthermore, it was impossible to survey all students
at the same time due to a limited number of survey administrators. Therefore,
time of day could have had an influence on the demeanor of the students,
causing possible misrepresentations of their true self-esteem.� Another variable of possible influence to
the research was the irregularity of the school weeks the surveys were
administered. The first day of administering surveys was a Wednesday followed
by a half day on Thursday due to Parent-Teacher conferences and no school on
Friday. The second and final day of surveying was the following Monday of which
preceded their spring break and Easter. Therefore, the abnormal week structure
may have influenced the self-reports of esteem.
Moreover, the small size of
each class may have resulted in a lack of difference in scores. Ideally, a
larger number of students in each grade would have resulted in a more diverse
sample. Also, the sample size from each grade fluctuated from as low as 11 to
as high as 63. The low return rate of consent forms for some grades resulted in
fewer students being surveyed in those classes. Therefore, the statistics for
some grades may be more accurate than others.
Perhaps the most influential
limitation of this study is that the participants surveyed were children. First
of all, even though the measures were directed towards their age groups, some
participants were still unable to completely understand the statements to which
they were responding. Although some students asked for clarification, it is
impossible to know if all the students truly comprehended the statements.
Furthermore, some students may not have acknowledged the importance of the
survey and therefore, not taken it seriously. Finally, children are generally
eager to please as well as be helpful. This eagerness may have influenced them
to respond with more desirable answers. In light of all these things, the
possible limitations are to be expected.
Although limitations were
encountered and the research did not support the hypothesis, the findings of
this study are nevertheless important to the body of self-esteem research. The
possibility of a discrepancy between small non-transition and larger transition
schools could be an area of focus for further research in self-esteem.� A comparison of the two types of schools would
be beneficial in determining whether or not the difference in educational
settings results in a disparity in self-esteem scores. Further study on
self-esteem in young children would also be desirable since the body of
research is lacking in this area.
�
References
Al-Sendi, A. M., Bahrain, S.
P., & Musaiger, A. O. (2004). Body weight perception among Bahraini
adolescents. Child: Care, Health &
Development, 30(4), 369-376.
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D.,
& Akert, R. M. (2005). Social
psychology (5th ed.). Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Baharudin, R., & Luster,
T. (1998). Factors related to the quality of the home environment and
children�s achievement. Journal of Family
Issues, 19(4), 375-403.
Burnett, P. (1996). Gender
and grade differences in elementary school children�s descriptive and
evaluative self-statements and self-esteem. School
Psychology International, 17(2), 159-170.
Cantin, S., & Boivin, M.
(2004). Change and stability in children�s social network and self perceptions
during transition from elementary to junior high school. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(6), 561-570.
Christoffersen, M. N.
(1994). A follow-up study of longterm effects of unemployment on children: Loss
of self-esteem and self-destructive behavior among adolescents. Childhood: A Global Journal of Child
Research, 2(4), 212-220.
Davies, J., & Brember,
I. (1999). Boys outperforming girls: An 8-year cross-sectional study of
attainment and self-esteem in year 6. Educational
Psychology, 19(1), 5-17.
Demo, D. H. (1992). The
self-concept over time: Research issues and directions. Annual Review of Sociology, 18, 303-326.
Direnfeld, G. (2003). Going
for the brass ring: Developing self-esteem in children. Healthy Weight Journal. 17(5), 73-75.
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A.,
Flanagan, C. A., Miller, C., Reuman, D. A., & Yee, D. (1989).
Self-concepts, domain values, and self-esteem: Relations and changes at early
adolescence. Journal of Personality, 57(2),
283-310
Frost, J., & McKelvie,
S. (2004). Self-esteem and body satisfaction in male and female elementary
school, high school, and university students. Sex Roles, 51(1/2), 45-54.
G�din, K. G., &
Hammarstr�m, A. (2003). Do changes in the psychosocial school environment
influence pupils� health development? Results from a three-year follow-up
study. Scandinavian Journal of Public
Health, 31(3), 169-178.
Gusella, J., Clark, S.,
& Van Roosmalen, E. (2004). Body image self-evaluation colouring lens:
Comparing the ornamental and instrumental views of adolescent girls with eating
disorders. European Eating Disorders
Review. 12(1), 223-229.
Hare, B. R. (1977). Racial
and socioeconomic variations in preadolescent area-specific and general
self-esteem. International Journal of
Intercultural Relations, 1(3), 31-51.
Hargreaves, D., & Tiggemann,
M. (2003). The effect of �thin ideal� television commercials on body
dissatisfaction and schema activation during early adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32(5),
367-373.
James, R. L. (n.d.). Correlation between adolescent self-esteem,
religiosity, and perceived family support. Unpublished manuscript, Loyola
University at New Orleans.�
Johansson, L., Lundh, L. G.,
& Andersson, G. (2005). Attentional bias for negative self-words in young
women: The role of thin ideal priming and body shape dissatisfaction. Personality & Individual Differences, 38(3),
73-84.
King, K. A., Vidourek, R.
A., Davis, B., & McClellan, W. (2002). Increasing self-esteem and school
connectedness through a multidimensional mentoring program. Journal of School Health, 72(7),
294-300.
Kjaerbye-Thygesen, A., Munk,
C., Ottesen, B., & Kruger, S. (2004, August 8). Why do slim women consider
themselves too heavy? A characterization of adult women considering their body
weight as too heavy. Wiley Periodicals, Article10.1002.
Retrieved February 10, 2005, from http://www.interscience.wiley.com
Kostanski, M. (2004). Body
image in teenage boys and girls. Brown
University Child & Adolescent Behavior Letter, 20(10), 5.
Kostanski, M., &
Gullone, E. (2004). Current conceptualization of body image dissatisfaction:
Have we got it wrong? Journal of Child
Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 45(7), 1317-1319.
Kuther, T. L., &
McDonald, E. (2004, Spring). Early adolescents� experiences with, and views of,
Barbie. Adolescence.
Laible, D. J., Carlo, G.,
& Roesch, S. C. (2004). Pathways to self-esteem in late adolescence: the
role of parent and peer attachment, empathy, and social behaviors. Journal of Adolescence, 27,703-716.
Lent, S. A., &
Figueira-McDonough, J. (2002). Gender and poverty: Self-esteem among elementary
school children. Journal of Children
& Poverty, 8(1), 5-22.
Longmore, M. A., Manning, W.
D., Giordano, P. C., & Rudolph, J. L. (2004). Self-esteem, depressive
symptoms, and adolescents� sexual onset. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 67(3), 279-295.
Miller, C. T., & Downey,
K. T. (1999). A meta-analysis of heavyweight and self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 3(1),
68-84.
Mori, M. (1999). The
influence of father-daughter relationship and girls� sex-roles on girls�
self-esteem. Archives of Women�s Mental
Health, 2(1), 45-48.
Owens, T. J. (1993).
Accentuate the positive and the negative: Rethinking the use of positive
self-esteem, self-deprecation, and self-confidence. Social Psychology Quarterly, 56,288-299.
Parker, J. G., Low, C. M.,
Walker, A. R., & Gamm, B. K. (2005).�
Friendship jealousy in young adolescents: Individual differences and
links to sex, self-esteem, aggression, and social adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 41(1),
235-250.
Robins, R. W., Trzesniewski,
K. H., Tracy, J. L., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2002). Global self
esteem across the life span. Psychology
and Aging, 17(3), 423-434.
Scheffler, T., & Naus,
P. (1999). The relationship between fatherly affirmation and a woman�s
self-esteem, fear of intimacy, comfort with womanhood and comfort with
sexuality. Canadian Journal of Human
Sexuality, 8(1), 39-46.
Slicker, E. K., Patton, M.,
& Fuller, D. K. (2004). Parenting dimensions and adolescent sexual
initiation: Using self-esteem, academic aspiration, and substance use as
mediators. Journal of Youth Studies, 7(3),
295-314.
Smith, S. (2001). Starving
self-esteem. Psychology Today, 34(3),
30.
Spencer, J. M., Zimet, G.
D., Aalsma, M. C., & Orr, D. P. (2002). Self-esteem as a predictor of
initiation of coitus in early adolescents. Pediatrics,
109(4), 581-584.
Twenge, J. M., &
Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: A
cross-temporal meta-analysis. Personality
& Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 321-344.
University of Maryland.
(2004). The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Retrieved January 30, 2005 from
University of Maryland, Department of Sociology: Department of Sociology web
site: http://www.bsos.umd.edu/socy.grad/socpsy_rosenberg.html
Wells, L E., & Marwell,
G. (1976). Self-esteem: Its
conceptualization and measurement. Beverly Hills: Sage.
Whitbeck, L. B., Simons, R.
L., Conger, R. D., Lorenz, F. O., Huck, S., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (1991).
Family economic hardship, parental support, and adolescent self-esteem. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54(4),
353-363.
Appendix A
Rosenberg�s Self-Esteem Scale
Instructions:
Below are a series of statements dealing with your general feelings about
yourself. Circle the response that indicates the extent to which you agree with
each of the following statements, using the scale below. There are no right or
wrong answers to the test:
|
|
Strongly
Agree
|
Agree
|
Disagree
|
Strongly
Disagree
|
1.
|
I feel that I'm a person of worth, at least on an
equal plane with others.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
2.
|
I feel that I have a number of good qualities.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
3.
|
All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
4.
|
I am able to do things as well as most other people.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
5.
|
I feel I do not have much to be proud of.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
6.
|
I take a positive attitude toward myself.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
7.
|
On the whole, I am satisfied with myself.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
8.
|
I wish I could have more respect for myself.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
9.
|
I certainly feel useless at times.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
10.
|
At times I think I am no good at all.
|
SA
|
A
|
D
|
SD
|
Appendix
B
Rosenberg�s
Self-Esteem Scale: Elementary Version
Please Listen for Directions!
Remember: there are no right or
wrong answers!
|
|
Oh Yeah!
|
Yeah, Sometimes.
|
No,
Not Really.
|
No Way!
|
1.
|
I feel I am as important as others.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
2.
|
I feel there are a lot of good things about me.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
3.
|
I tend to feel that I am a loser.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
4.
|
I feel I can do things as well as most other people.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
5.
|
I feel I don�t have much to be proud of.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
6.
|
I have good feelings about myself.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
7.
|
Most of the time I am happy with myself.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
8.
|
I wish I could appreciate myself more.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
9.
|
I feel useless sometimes.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
10.
|
At times I think I am no good at all.
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
Appendix C
Informed Consent
STATEMENT OF INFORMED CONSENT�������������������������
This research project will require
participants to complete a ten question self-report, which will be completed
during the time of their guidance session in the classroom.� This questionnaire asks the respondent to
circle the number that indicates the degree to which he/she agrees with each of
the ten statements.� The time that is
required to complete this is ten minutes.�
We are interested in determining
characteristics of childhood perceptions.�
Your child�s participation will help in this matter.� There are no known risks or ill effects from
participating in this study.
You have the right to refuse your
child�s participation in this study or to withdraw your consent at any time
during the study without prejudice from the investigator.� If you would like to know more information
about the study prior to signing the consent form, you may contact Jennifer
Strange at [email protected].� Additionally, you are entitled to be
informed as to the results of this study upon its completion and may contact us
after May 1, 2005 via the previously provided email address or by the address
below.
Inasmuch as the names of the
participants are not relevant to the analysis of the data, the manner in which
the data is both gathered and stored will ensure the anonymity of each
participant. Your child�s name will NOT be used anywhere in the data collection
or analysis.
Please be assured that all
information will be processed in keeping with professional standards.� Thank you in advance for your time and
cooperation.�
************************************************************************
I have read the above statement of
informed consent and I agree that my child may participate in this study.� My questions about procedures and/or purpose
of the experiment have been satisfactorily answered.� I understand that I will receive a copy of this consent form to
keep for future reference should I desire it.�
_____________________________
Signature
Abby Dauer, Nicole Neuenschwander,
Jenni Strange
Huntington College
2303 College Ave.
Huntington,
IN� 46750
Appendix
D
Junior
High & High School Script
INSTRUCTIONS FOR TAKING THIS SURVEY
Hi, our names are _____________. We
want to thank you for helping us out today in our research. We will be handing
out a survey with ten statements.
There are a series
of statements dealing with your general feelings about yourself.
Circle the response that indicates the
extent to which you agree with each of the statements, using the scale. There
are no right or wrong answers to the test.
If you are a male, please write an M at
the top of your paper. If you are a female, please put an F at the top of your
paper.
When you are done with the survey,
please turn your paper over and we will pick it up. Thank you.
CLOSING REMARKS