Layers of HIV Stigma: The Influence of High-risk Characteristics on PerceptionsKerry A. Newness and Lenore Szuchman*
|
Vignette Question |
M |
SE |
F |
df, error |
η2 |
Responsibility |
3.17 |
.10 |
41.13* |
6, 576 |
.30 |
Verbal Assault |
3.30 |
.07 |
16.39* |
6, 576 |
.15 |
Physical Assault |
3.04 |
.07 |
21.68* |
6, 576 |
.18 |
Coping Ability |
2.30 |
.06 |
62.92* |
6, 576 |
.40 |
Society Acceptance |
2.95 |
.08 |
24.28* |
6, 576 |
.20 |
Trouble with Law |
3.11 |
.06 |
130.72* |
6, 576 |
.58 |
Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations for All Vignette Conditions and Questions
Vignette Questions |
||||||||||||||
Responsible |
Verbal |
Physical |
Coping |
Society |
Law |
|||||||||
Condition |
M |
SD |
M |
SD |
M |
SM |
M |
SD |
M |
SD |
M |
SD |
||
HIV+ |
3.86a |
1.18 |
3.02a |
1.13 |
2.53a |
1.07 |
3.53b |
1.06 |
3.46a |
1.04 |
2.11a |
1.04 |
||
Gay |
4.07a |
1.18 |
2.94a |
1.27 |
2.73a |
1.17 |
4.07a |
0.97 |
3.58a |
1.14 |
1.91a |
1.06 |
||
Drug User |
2.78b |
1.54 |
2.74a |
1.15 |
2.60a |
1.05 |
2.91c |
1.17 |
3.18a |
1.14 |
3.73b |
1.10 |
||
HIV+/Gay |
3.82a |
1.23 |
3.46b |
1.18 |
3.14b |
1.08 |
3.47b |
1.10 |
2.80b |
1.18 |
2.01a |
1.06 |
||
HIV+/Drug |
2.47b |
1.65 |
3.49b |
1.06 |
3.29b |
1.03 |
2.09d |
0.99 |
2.61c |
1.15 |
4.03b |
0.96 |
||
Gay/Drug |
2.75b |
1.50 |
3.49b |
1.06 |
3.28b |
1.03 |
2.81c |
1.21 |
2.74b |
1.19 |
3.82b |
0.98 |
||
HIV+/Gay/Drug |
2.45b |
1.61 |
3.90c |
1.06 |
3.69c |
0.99 |
2.01d |
0.91 |
2.29c |
1.20 |
4.16c |
0.81 |
A Tukey post hoc analysis was conducted for each of the six vignette questions to determine whether the layering of high-risk HIV characteristics was perceived significantly more negatively than single high-risk characteristics and whether the combination of all three high-risk characteristics was perceived to be significantly more negative than the combination of two high-risk characteristics.
The post hoc analysis for the responsibility vignette question indicated no significant difference between responses for two of the three single high-risk conditions (i.e., gay and HIV+) and the gay/HIV+ condition; however, the remaining four conditions, all of which included the drug user characteristic (i.e., drug user, gay/drug user, HIV+/drug user, and HIV+/gay/drug user), were perceived as significantly more negative.
The Tukey post hoc analysis for the verbal assault and physical assault vignette questions, as hypothesized, indicated no significant difference in response among the single, high-risk characteristic conditions; the targets with the layering of two high-risk characteristics were perceived to be significantly more negative, and the target with all three high-risk characteristics was perceived to be significantly more negative than the other six targets.
The Tukey post hoc analysis for the coping vignette question suggested that the targets in the HIV+ and HIV+/gay conditions were perceived as less able to cope than in the gay condition. The targets in the gay/drug user and drug user conditions were perceived to be significantly less able to cope than the characters in the HIV+ and HIV+/gay condition, and the target in the HIV+/gay/drug user and HIV+/drug user conditions were perceived to be less able to cope than the other five targets.
The post hoc analysis for the societal acceptance vignette questions suggested no significant difference in the conditions with single high-risk characteristics (i.e., HIV+, gay, and drug user). The HIV+/gay and gay/drug user targets were perceived to be significantly less accepted by society than the single, high-risk targets, and the HIV+/drug user and HIV+/gay/drug user targets were perceived to be significantly less accepted by society than the other five targets.
The post hoc analysis for the vignette question addressing the character’s likelihood for being in trouble with the law indicated no significant difference between responses for two of the three single high-risk conditions (i.e., gay and HIV+) and the gay/HIV+ condition. The drug user, gay/drug user, and HIV+/drug user targets were perceived to be significantly more likely to be in trouble with the law, and the target in the condition with the layering of all three high-risk characteristics was perceived to be significantly more likely to be in trouble with the law than the other six conditions.
A between-subjects MANOVA was conducted to determine gender differences in perceptions of responsibility and social acceptance for the HIV+ condition, F(2, 94) = 772.10, p < .001. Both univariate ANOVAs were significant. Women (M = 4.09, SD = 1.02) perceived the target from the HIV+ vignette to be significantly more responsible than men (M = 3.55, SD = 1.07), F(2, 94) = 5.26, p < .05. Women perceived the character from the HIV+ vignette to be significantly more accepted by society than men, F(2, 94) = 7.52, p < .01.
A between-subjects MANOVA was conducted to determine gender differences on the Stigma and Belief survey scales, F(4, 92) = 4.84, p < .01. The univariate ANOVA for coercive public policies indicated that women (M = 3.56, SD = .54) disagreed with the policies significantly more than men (M = 3.16, SD = .63), F(1, 95) = 11.58, p < .01. There were no significant gender differences in casual contact beliefs or in negative feelings toward people living with HIV, and high-risk group beliefs.
To determine what factors may have contributed to gender differences in perception of societal acceptance and responsibility in the HIV-positive vignette, separate multiple regressions were conducted for women and men using as independent variables the four Stigma and Belief Survey scales and whether or not the participants knew someone with HIV. For women, the prediction for societal acceptance was significant, adjusted R 2 = .21, p = .004. Significant predictors were low reports of negative feelings, low support for coercive public policy, and knowledge of how the virus can and cannot be contracted through casual contact. (See Table 3 for analysis summary). For men, the prediction for societal acceptance was not significant. The prediction for responsibility was significant for women, adjusted R 2 = .12, p = .05. Responses on the casual contact belief scale were a significant predictor of responsibility for women (See Table 4 for analysis summary). The prediction for responsibility was not significant for men.
Table 3. Regression Analysis Summary for Societal Acceptance
Women |
Men |
|||||||||||
Predictor |
B |
SEM |
β |
t |
p |
B |
SEM |
β |
t |
p |
||
Negative Feelings |
.61 |
.23 |
.45 |
2.70 |
.01 |
.78 |
.38 |
.41 |
2.05 |
.05 |
||
Public Policy |
-.68 |
.28 |
-.39 |
-2.42 |
.02 |
-.16 |
.34 |
-.09 |
-.47 |
.64 |
||
Casual Contact |
.30 |
.15 |
.27 |
1.99 |
.05 |
-.27 |
.22 |
-.24 |
-1.25 |
.22 |
||
High Risk Group |
-.04 |
.22 |
-.03 |
-.20 |
.85 |
.24 |
.31 |
.12 |
.77 |
.44 |
||
Know HIV |
.05 |
.25 |
.03 |
.21 |
.84 |
.24 |
.41 |
.10 |
.60 |
.55 |
Table 4. Regression Analysis Summary for Responsibility
Women |
Men |
|||||||||||
Predictor |
B |
SEM |
β |
t |
p |
B |
SEM |
β |
t |
p |
||
Negative Feelings |
-.11 |
.26 |
-.07 |
-.42 |
.68 |
.94 |
.46 |
.40 |
2.05 |
.05 |
||
Public Policy |
-.33 |
.32 |
-.17 |
-1.03 |
.31 |
-.10 |
.40 |
-.05 |
-.24 |
.81 |
||
Casual Contact |
.45 |
.17 |
.37 |
2.59 |
.01 |
-.60 |
.26 |
-.43 |
-2.30 |
.03 |
||
High Risk Group |
.31 |
.25 |
.18 |
1.27 |
.21 |
.13 |
.37 |
.05 |
.35 |
.73 |
||
Know HIV |
.42 |
.28 |
.21 |
1.51 |
.14 |
.09 |
.49 |
.03 |
.18 |
.86 |
Of the participants who knew someone living with HIV, 24 were women (72.7%) and 9 were men (27.3%). These frequencies were significantly different from chance, χ 2 = (1, N = 97) = 5.23, p < .05. Women’s responses on the coercive public policy scale (M = 3.56, SD = .54) were significantly correlated with whether they knew someone living with HIV, r(53) = .31, p < .05. Men’s responses on the coercive public policy scale (M = 3.16, SD = .63) were not significantly correlated with whether they knew someone with HIV, r(40) = .60, p = .08. Negative feelings toward people living with HIV were significantly correlated with responses on coercive public policies for women (M = 3.32, SD = .70), r(53) = .39, p < .01, and men (M = 3.33, SD = .57), r(40) = .52, p < .001. Additionally, men’s responses on the casual contact belief scale (M = 3.90, SD = .94) were significantly correlated with responses on the coercive public policy scale, r(40) = .49, p < .01.
Evidence supporting the layering theory of high-risk HIV characteristics suggests that multiple high-risk behaviors are associated with increased experience of social stigmatization. Results from the univariate analysis regarding the character’s responsibility suggest that participants perceived IV drug use to be significantly more irresponsible than the behaviors depicted in the vignette conditions without IV drug use. The results also suggest that perceptions of responsibility are not dependent on the layering of high-risk behaviors; instead, perceptions may have been influenced by the legal constraints. More negative perceptions of drug use than for positive serostatus are consistent with results from Capitanio et al. (1999). Perhaps the characters in the vignettes who used intravenous drugs were thought to be significantly more irresponsible because the act of using hard drugs is life-threatening. Furthermore, taking part in recreational drug use is a personal choice, whereas being gay has many biological components and contracting HIV can occur through other means than direct sexual contact.
An individual’s ability to cope with his or her life situation in response to the diagnosis of a terminal disease is critical for physical well-being. The post hoc test used to examine the vignette question about the character’s ability to cope with his life situation yielded more variation than the other vignette questions. The character portrayed in the gay vignette condition was thought to be more able to cope than the characters in the other vignette conditions, which may suggest that participants did not see homosexual orientation as a significant life stressor. The characters in the vignette conditions that included IV-drug use were thought to be the least able to cope with their life situation. Intravenous drug use, therefore, could have been perceived by participants as a negative means for coping with life situations. Moreover, participants thought the characters in the vignette conditions with the combination of IV-drug use and positive HIV status would be able to cope even less than vignette conditions with just IV-drug use. These results provide partial support for the layering of stigma hypothesis.
In general terms, stigma is experienced by various individuals because society is not accepting of a physical attribute. Results from the Tukey post hoc test for the societal acceptance vignette question provide partial support for the layering of stigma hypothesis. Specifically, the targets with only one high-risk characteristic were perceived to be significantly more accepted. This suggests that participants did not perceive any difference between the gay, HIV-positive, or IV-drug use high-risk characteristics, but that the combination of characteristics would have a negative influence on acceptance. Of the vignette conditions with two or all three high-risk characteristics, participants thought an individual with the combination of IV-drug use and HIV-positive status would experience the least societal acceptance.
Verbal assault and physical assault are two specific manifestations of social stigmatization. The results from the Tukey post hoc tests for verbal and physical abuse vignette conditions provide evidence to support the hypothesis that participants would perceive the multiple high-risk characteristics as significantly more negative than single high-risk characteristics. Compared with the character described as having only one high-risk characteristic, participants thought the character depicted as having the combination of two high-risk characteristics was significantly more likely to be verbally and physically assaulted by individuals in society. Moreover, the character with all three high-risk characteristics was thought to be significantly more likely to be verbally and physically assaulted than the characters with two high-risk characteristics.
Although the likelihood of being in trouble with the law should be perceived as independent from HIV-positive status and homosexual orientation, results suggest otherwise. The character from the vignette condition with the layering of all three high-risk characteristics was thought to be the most likely in trouble with the law. Characters in vignette conditions without the IV-drug use variable were perceived to be the least likely in trouble with the law; participants’ perceptions, therefore, were influenced by the negative sanctions associated with IV-drug use. These results suggest that the severity of stigma experienced by individuals living with HIV may vary based on both the layering and type of high-risk characteristic. If there are legal implications associated with the high-risk characteristic, such as IV-drug use, then the HIV-positive individual may experience greater stigmatization.
The results of the gender analysis suggest that women perceived the character in the HIV only vignette to be more responsible for things that happen to him than did the men. Casual contact beliefs and knowledge of the means of transmission, according to the regression analysis, significantly predicted this perceived responsibility for women. The direction of this regression was positive for women (i.e., the more informed on causal contact, the more responsible the participant perceived the target). Unfortunately, this particular question was ambiguous, for the vignette included both responsible and irresponsible behaviors. Contracting the HIV virus may be interpreted as irresponsible behavior; whereas, being tested may be seen as responsible behavior. Women who were informed concerning types of casual contact, therefore, may have thought the target was responsible for being tested. The target’s romantic partner in the HIV-positive only (i.e., not gay) condition was a woman, so women many have been able to relate to the vignettes more than the men. Furthermore, men and women were equally knowledgeable about the means of HIV transmission and did not differ in their beliefs on the casual contact scale. In the current sample, more women knew someone with HIV than men; as a result, contact theory (Allport, 1954) may explain women’s more positive attributions.
In addition to having significantly more positive responses on the responsibility vignette question, women perceived the HIV-positive character to be significantly more accepted by society than men. The negative feelings toward people living with HIV responses were significant predictors of societal acceptance for women; therefore, they may have been projecting their personal feelings for societal acceptance. Additionally, men were more likely to favor the coercive public policies than women, and they thought society would be less accepting overall. Ratings on the level of societal acceptance, therefore, may suggest that women are more likely to be compassionate or empathetic toward individuals living with HIV.
More women in this sample knew someone living with HIV than men, and there was a significant difference in beliefs of coercive public policies. Women’s responses on public policy issues correlated with their knowing someone with HIV, but men’s responses did not correlate with their knowing someone with HIV. For men, the knowledge of how the virus may be transmitted and contracted correlated with their responses on the coercive public policy items. Whether women agreed with negative public policies, therefore, may have been decided based on feelings and emotions, whereas men’s responses for public policy may have been based on knowledge of transmission. Stigma, in the form of social avoidance and discrimination, may be related to the lack of knowing someone with HIV or being able to relate to seropositive individuals on a personal level. Interventional programs designed to reduce the stigma associated with HIV may consider combining an informational component with an emotional component that allows participants to interact with and get to know someone with HIV.
The current study had several limitations. First, the college sample may be especially aware of the means of contraction of HIV. However, stigma associated with the HIV virus is driven by fear and lack of knowledge about the means of contraction. Results may not, therefore, be representative of the general U.S. population. Additionally, the participants were students living in South Florida, where the number of individuals living with HIV is greater than in some other parts of the country. Participants’ perceptions of the layering of stigma may have been influenced by greater exposure to people living with HIV. Furthermore, the demographic information collected in the current study did not include a question regarding participants’ sexual orientation. The manipulation of homosexual orientation as a high-risk characteristic within the vignettes could have been perceived differently by participants identifying themselves as heterosexual, bisexual, or homosexual. The methodology of the current study manipulated variables using vignettes; however, there are certain limitations associated with this method (Hughes, & Huby, 2004). Results, consequently, provide insight into the perceptions of the layering of stigma, but cannot determine actual levels of stigma experienced by individuals living with the HIV virus.
The current study examined the layering of HIV stigma through the manipulation of HIV status, homosexual orientation, and intravenous drug use as high-risk characteristics, but there may be other characteristics that influence perceptions of individuals living with HIV. For example, marital infidelity or sexual promiscuity may influence perceptions of HIV-positive individuals. The vignettes in the current study only included a male character; however, HIV-positive females may experience different types or severity of stigmatization. Future studies examining the layering of HIV stigma would benefit from including both genders and more high-risk characteristics. Additional research on the factors that contribute to the stigmatizing behavior toward individuals living with HIV is necessary.
The stigmatization experienced by people living with HIV is a pervasive problem despite the general population’s conception that the severity of stigmatization has decreased over the past 20 years (Samuels, 2008). In fact, an HIV-positive homeless man from Texas was charged and sentenced in 2008 to 35 years in prison for assault with a deadly weapon when he spat on a police officer (Kovach, 2008). This incident exemplifies how an individual may stigmatize because he or she fears contraction, even knowing the means of transmission. In this recent case, the man was homeless, effectively layering his HIV stigma with another highly stigmatized characteristic. Thus the implications for work on the layering of stigma are very real and worthy of further study.
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Altman, D. (1986). AIDS in the mind of America. Garden City, NY: Anchor Press Doubleday.
Capitanio, J. P., & Herek, G. M. (1999). AIDS-related stigma and attitudes toward injection drug users among black and white Americans. The American Behavioral Scientist, 42, 1148-1161.
Center for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC]. (2005). HIV/AIDS surveillance report 2005, 17, 1-46: Atlanta, GA.
Courtenay-Quirk, C., Wolitski, R. J., Parsons, J. T., Gomez, C. A., & the Seropositive Urban Men’s Study Team. (2006). Is HIV/AIDS stigma dividing the gay community? Perceptions of HIV-positive men who have sex with men. AIDS Education and Prevention, 18, 56-67.
Crandall, C. S. (1991). Multiple stigma and AIDS: Illness stigma and attitudes toward homosexuals and IV drug users in AIDS-related stigmatization. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 1, 165-172.
Domek, G. J. (2006). Social consequences of antiretroviral therapy: Preparing for the unexpected futures of HIV-positive children. Lancet, 367, 1367-1369.
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management of spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc.
Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1993). Public Reactions to AIDS in the United States: A Second Decade of Stigma. American Journal of Public Health, 83, 574-577.
Hergovich, A., Ratky, E., & Stollreiter, M. (2003). Attitudes toward HIV-positives in dependence on their sexual orientation. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 62, 37-44.
Hughes, R., & Huby, M. (2004). The construction and interpretation of vignette in social research. Social Work and Social Sciences Review, 11, 36-51.
Kovach, G. C. (2008, May 16). Prison for man with H.I.V. who spit on a police officer, The New York Times.
Mateu-Gelabert, P., Maslow, C., Flom, P. L., Sandoval, M., Bolyard, M., & Friedman, S. R. (2005). Keeping it together: Stigma, response, and perception of risk in relationships between drug injectors and crack smokers, and other community residents. AIDS Care, 17, 802-813.
O’Cleirigh, C., Ironson, G., Antoni, M., Fletcher, M. A., McGuffey, L., Balbin, E., Schneiderman, N., Solomon, G. (2003). Emotional expression and depth processing of trauma and their relation to long-term survival in patients with HIV/AIDS. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 225-235.
Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., Yeadon, C., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (2004). A dual-process model of reactions to perceived stigma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 436-452.
Samuels, J. M. (2008, May 27). Saliva and HIV [Letter to the editor]. The New York Times.
Seacat, J. D., Hirschman, R., & Mickelson, K. D. (2007). Attributions of HIV onset controllability, emotional reactions, and helping intentions: Implicit effects of victim sexual orientation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1442-1461.
Simoni, J. M., Frick, P. A., & Huang, B. (2006). Longitudinal evaluation of a social support model of medication adherence among HIV-positive men and women on antiviral therapy. Health Psychology, 25, 74-81.
Swendeman, D., Rotheram-Borus, M. J., Comulada, S., Weiss, R., & Ramos, M. E. (2006). Predictors of HIV-related stigma among young people living with HIV. Health Psychology, 25, 501-509.
Weiner, B., Perry, R. P., & Magnusson, J. (1988). An attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 738-748.
Yang, H., Li, H., Stanton, B., Fang, X., Lin, D., & Naar-King, S. (2006). HIV-related knowledge, stigma, and willingness to disclose: A meditation analysis. AIDS Care, 18, 717-724.
|