Relationships between hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity and risk-taking propensity in an international sample of undergraduate students.Thomas Richardson
|
Scale |
Chronbach's Alpha |
HCL-32 |
|
Total |
.814 |
Active/Elevated |
.777 |
Risk-Taking/Irritable |
.630 |
BIS |
|
Total |
.847 |
Attention |
.716 |
Motor |
.688 |
Self-Control |
.756 |
Cognitive Complexity |
.434 |
Perseverance |
.134 |
Cognitive Instability |
.502 |
DOSPERT |
|
Total |
.844 |
Ethical |
.566 |
Financial |
.771 |
Health/Safety |
.644 |
Recreational |
.822 |
Social |
.606 |
Score ranges
HCL-32 total scores ranged from 4-31 out of 32, with a mean of 18.2 (SD=5.05). BIS total scores ranged from 39
to 104 out of 120, with a mean of 64.96 (SD=11.10). DOSPERT likelihood total scores ranged from 47 to 182 out of
210, with a mean of 101.5 (SD=21.74).
Overall correlations
The HCL-32 total was significantly positively correlated with the DOSPERT total; r=.26, p<.001, one-tailed,
and BIS total; r=.39, p<.001, one-tailed. The DOSPERT total and BIS total were also significantly positive correlated;
r=.378, p<.001, one-tailed.
Multiple Regression Analyses
General hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity
A multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 total as the dependent variable and the BIS subscales as the
predictors. Overall, there was a statistically significant relationship between the BIS subscales and the HCL-32
total- F=9.376, p<.001 (R2 =.191). Table 2 presents the standardised coefficients for each subscale of the
BIS as the predictors, and the subscales of the HCL-32 as the dependents. As table 2 demonstrates, the attention
and motor impulsivity subscales of the BIS were both independent predictors of the HCL-32 total.
Table 2: Standardized Coefficients for BIS subscales as predictors and HCL-32 subscales as dependents
Dependent- HCL-32 |
|||
Predictor- BIS |
Total |
Active/Elevated |
Risk-Taking/Irritable |
Attention |
.191* |
.094 |
.186* |
Motor |
.237*** |
.097 |
.248*** |
Self-Control |
-.117 |
.194* |
.046 |
Cognitive Complexity |
.078 |
.063 |
.102 |
Perseverance |
.118 |
.132 |
.089 |
Cognitive Instability |
.087 |
.139 |
0.016 |
*= p<.05
**= p<.01
***= p<.001
Active/Elevated hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity
A standard multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale as the dependent variable
and the BIS subscales as the predictors. Overall there was a statistically significant relationship between the
BIS subscales and the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale- F=4.013, p<.001 (R2 =.092). As table 2 demonstrates,
the only significant independent predictor of the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale was the Self Control subscale
of the BIS.
Risk-Taking/Irritable hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity
A standard multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale as the dependent variable
and the BIS subscales as the predictors. Overall there was a statistically significant relationship between the
BIS subscales and the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale- F=11.475, p<.001 (R2 =.224). As table 2 demonstrates,
the Attention and Motor subscales of the BIS were significant independent predictors of scores on the HCL-32
Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale.
General hypomanic symptoms and risk-taking propensity
A standard multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 Total as the dependent variable and the DOSPERT subscales
as the predictors. Overall there was a statistically significant relationship between the DOSPERT subscales and
the HCL-32 Total- F=4.507, p<.001 (R2 = .086). Table 3 presents the standardised coefficients for each subscale
of the DOSPERT as the predictors, and the subscales of the HCL-32 as the dependents. As table 3 demonstrates,
none of the subscales of the DOSPERT were significant independent predictors of scores on the HCL-32 total.
Table 3: Standardized Coefficients for DOSPERT subscales as predictors and HCL-32 subscales as dependents
|
|
Dependent- HCL-32 |
|
Predictor- DOSPERT |
Total |
Active/Elevated |
Risk-Taking/Irritable |
Ethical |
.117 |
.032 |
.182* |
Financial |
-.019 |
.001 |
-.038 |
Health/Safety |
.152 |
-.018 |
.270*** |
Recreational |
.132 |
.141 |
.082 |
Social |
-.025 |
-.009 |
-.071 |
*= p<.05
**= p<.01
***= p<.001
Active/Elevated hypomanic symptoms and risk-taking propensity
A standard multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale as the dependent variable
and the DOSPERT subscales as the predictors. Overall, there was not a statistically significant relationship
between the DOSPERT subscales and the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale- F=1.006, p>.05 (R2 =.021). As table
3 demonstrates, none of the subscales of the DOSPERT were significant independent predictors of scores on the
HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale.
Risk-Taking/Irritable hypomanic symptoms and risk-taking propensity
A standard multiple regression was conducted with the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable as the dependent variable and
the DOSPERT subscales as the predictors. Overall, there was a statistically significant relationship between
the DOSPERT subscales and the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale- F=9.362, p<.001 (R2=.163.). Table 6 presents
the standardised coefficients for each subscale. As table 3 demonstrates, the Ethical and Health/Safety subscales
of the DOSPERT were significant independent predictors of scores on the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale.
Demographic interactions
Analyses were conducted on all statistically significant predictors to determine the effects of demographic variables
of age, nationality and gender on relationships. Three significant differences were found. Specifically, there
was a significant effect of age on the relationship between the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale and the
BIS Motor subscale; t=2.205, p<.05, with the relationship being stronger with increasing age. There was also
a significant effect of age on the relationship between the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale and the DOSPERT
Ethical subscale; t=-3.253, p<.05, with the relationship being weaker with increasing age. Finally, there
was a significant effect of gender on the relationship between the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale and
the DOSPERT Health/Safety subscale; t=-2.019, p<.05, with the relationship being stronger for women.
This study examined the relationship between hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity and risk taking propensity in an international sample of undergraduate students. The results suggest overall strong positive correlations between symptoms of hypomania and impulsivity and risk-taking propensity in this non-clinical population. This suggests strong ‘dose-response’ relationships, with increased impulsivity being associated with increasingly severe hypomanic symptoms. This is in line with previous findings of positive correlations between manic severity and impulsivity (Benazzi, 2007a; Lewis et al., 2009; Swann et al., 2007, 2008).
Whilst total scores were significantly correlated, multiple regression analyses were required to determine which specific aspects of hypomania were related to which specific aspects of impulsivity in this population. The analyses suggested that when the other subscales were held constant, only a few aspects of impulsivity were independently related to hypomanic symptoms. For example, although total BIS score predicted HCL-32 total and Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale, only the Attention and Motor subscales were independently related to these hypomania subscales. This is in line with DSM-IV-TR criteria for a hypomanic episode, for example criterion B5 (APA, 2000, p. 365) states that a potential symptom is ‘Distractibility (i.e. attention too easily drawn to unimportant or irrelevant external stimuli), which is reflected in the Attention subscale of the BIS. Previous research has also documented impaired attention in those with bipolar disorder (Swann et al., 2009b). Also the Motor subscale of the BIS would appear to capture an important aspect of criterion B6, which states that ‘psychomotor agitation’ may be present (APA, 2000, p. 365). Swann et al (2009b) found that patients with bipolar disorder were poor at inhibiting rapid motor responses, suggesting that motor impulsivity may be an important part of manic sympomatology. The specific aspects of impulsivity measured by the BIS including Self-control, Cognitive Complexity, Perseverance and Cognitive Instability were not related to general hypomania and irritable and risk-taking symptoms in this sample. This suggests that, in non-clinical populations at least, general hypomanic symptoms and those of irritability and risk-taking are related to specific impulsive symptoms of attention and motor impulsivity.
Although the HCL-32 Active/Elevated subscale was significantly related to the overall BIS score, only the self-control subscale of the BIS was related to this aspect of hypomania. This relationship with self-control may be related to the potential symptoms specified in criterion B7 of the DSM-IV-TR such as unrestrained buying sprees and sexual indiscretions. Previous research has also documented poor self control associated with hypomania and bipolar disorder as demonstrated by excessive spending (Akiskal & Pinto, 1999), and increased sexual promiscuity (Dell’Osso et al., 2009). Thus, in non-clinical populations, hypomania elation may also be related to a lack of self-control and may result in unrestrained actions such as these. However, overall active and elevated aspects of hypomania do not appear to be as strongly related to impulsivity as general hypomanic symptoms and risk-taking and irritable symptoms. This suggests that risk-taking and irritable hypomanic symptoms are more impulsive than active or elevated symptoms. Thus, an irritable mood whilst hypomanic may be associated with greater impulsivity than when mood is elated. This may be important as it suggests that different mood presentations may require different treatments for the subsequent severity of accompanying impulsivity. However it is important to note that the present study examined symptoms in the general population, and this may not necessarily be the case for clinical hypomanic episodes and bipolar disorder.
The DOSPERT, measuring risk-taking propensity, was less strongly related to hypomania in this sample. The total HCL-32 score was significantly predicted by all the DOSPERT subscales combined, but no subscale individually. This suggests that general hypomanic symptoms are related to general increased risk-taking propensity, but no specific type of risk taking appears to be particularly elevated. However it is important to consider that the total scores may simply be more reliable than the individual subscales; as table 1 shows the chronbach’s alpha scores are higher for the total scores than the subscales. Therefore, in this non-clinical population, hypomanic symptoms are strongly related to the likelihood of engaging in potentially risky behaviours. This again is in line with DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria of “increased involvement in pleasurable activities that have high potential for painful consequences” (APA, 2000, p. 365), and is in accordance with previous research documenting negative consequences of hypomania such as increased marital disruption (Angst, 1998), and high comorbidity between bipolar disorder and impulse control disorders such as kleptomania (McElroy et al., 1991) and pathological gambling (McCormick et al., 1984). However it appears that no specific types of risk-taking, such as ethical or financial risks, are independently related to hypomania, but rather increased hypomania is related to a general increase in risk-taking propensity.
The Active/Elevated subscale of the HCL-32 was not significantly related to the subscales of the DOSPERT, whilst the HCL-32 Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale was. This suggests that risk-taking symptoms are rarely related to active and eleated symptoms in non-clinical populations, and are more commonly associated with risk taking and irritable hypomanic symptoms. Therefore risk-taking behaviours may be more common when the mood accompanying hypomania is irritable rather than elated. The Ethical and Health/Safety subscales were independently related to the HCL-32 risk-taking subscale. This suggests that ethical risks given by the DOSPERT, such as ‘Having an affair with a married woman’, and Health and Safety risks, such as ‘Driving a car without a seatbelt’ are particularly related to the irritable symptoms accompanying hypomania. This would be in line with DSM-IV-TR symptoms and previous research (Akiskal & Pinto, 1999), but it should be noted that the hypomania here probably represents sub-clinical hypomania.
It is interesting to note that some of the relationships between hypomania and impulsivity and risk-taking propensity described above were influenced by demographic variables. Increasing age was associated with a stronger relationship between the HCL Risk-Taking/Irritable subscale and the BIS motor scale, suggesting that psychomotor agitation association may be more pronounced and more strongly associated with an irritable mood in those of increasing age. On the other hand it appears that this subscale’s relationship with the DOSPERT ethical subscale was associated with age in the reverse direction, suggesting that hypomania may be associated with greater ethical risks, such as “Passing somebody else’s work as your own” in those of a younger age. Previous work suggests that age effects impulsivity and sensation seeking propensity (Steinberg et al., 2008). The current results demonstrate that this may also be the case for the relationship between impulsive and risk-taking symptoms and bipolar affective symptoms. Gender also affected the relationship between this subscale and the DOSPERT Health/Safety subscale, with a stronger relationship for women. This suggests that increasing hypomania symptoms, and specifically an irritable mood, may be more likely to result in individuals taking health and safety risks, such as ‘Walking home alone at night in an unsafe area of town’, in women than in men. This is in line with various pieces of research demonstrating a number of gender differences in bipolar disorder (Arnold, 2003). However it goes against research which suggests that men with bipolar disorder are more likely than women to experience comorbidities related to impulsivity and risk-taking such as alcohol use and gambling problems (Kawa et al., 2005). These results tentatively suggest that hypomania may be associated with impulsivity and risk-taking propensity in a different way for those of a differing age, and that gender may also affect the relationship. However, it should be noted that this study examined such symptoms in a non-clinical population, and also the low number of males in the sample may have affected statistical relationship with gender observed here.
Whilst previous research on clinical populations demonstrates the importance of impulsive personality traits in hypomania (Benazzi, 2007), this suggests that such a relationship exists in non clinical populations. Previous work suggests that such a relationship exists within the general population in terms of personality traits (Durban et al., 2009), and this study suggests that this may also be the case for sub-clinical hypomanic symptoms. However, the nature of causality in these results is unclear. It could be that increased impulsivity increases the severity of hypomania; previous research demonstrates that impulsivity predicts bipolar disorder at follow up (Kwapil et al., 2000). Equally, it could be that increased severity of hypomania increases subsequent impulsiveness; previous research demonstrates that impulsivity is a key symptom during manic episodes (Swann et al., 2001a). Related to this is whether the findings suggest that trait or state impulsivity is important in hypomania. Whilst the BIS generally measures trait impulsivity, it is possible that it is also picking up on state impulsivity during (probably subclinical) hypomanic episodes. Thus whilst this research may suggest that trait impulsivity is strong in those with hypomanic tendencies, it may also imply that hypomanic episodes are associated with state impulsivity, in line with previous research on the symptoms of mania (Swann et al., 2001a; Swann et al., 2001b). It is also possible that impulsivity represents both a trait and state component in hypomania, in line with previous research on bipolar II patients (Benazzi, 2007). Whether impulsivity is a trait or state, this research suggests that impulsivity and risk taking behaviours are related in a dose-response manner to hypomanic symptoms in non-clinical populations. Thus impulsivity and risk-taking propensity may need to be taken into consideration during the assessment and treatment of hypomania, in line with previous research suggesting that increased impulsivity increases the severity of manic symptoms (Swann et al., 2008; Swann et al., 2007).
A number of limitations of this study need to be considered. First, the sample consists of psychology undergraduate students, and as a result is predominantly female. Research has demonstrated that hypomania may be more prevalent in women (Angst et al., 2005b), and research also shows gender differences in impulsivity and risk-taking behaviours (D’Acremont & Van Der Linden, 2005). Second, as previously mentioned, it is important to understand that the sample is non-clinical, and thus the hypomanic symptoms measured here probably represent subclincial hypomanic symptoms, and thus it cannot be confidently suggested that DSM-IV-TR hypomanic episodes will be related to impulsivity and risk-taking behaviours in the same way. Third, the HCL-32 was developed to distinguish unipolar depression from bipolar II disorder, and thus is not intended for use in non-clinical populations. However, research has shown that the HCL-32 can detect hypomanic symptoms in non-clinical groups (Vieta et al., 2007). Similarly, as table 1 shows, whilst most of the chronbach’s alpha scores obtained with this sample were satisfactory, a number of subscales had low scores, questioning the reliability of their use in this current non-clinical sample. Finally, due to the number of multiple regressions calculated, there is an inflated experimentwise alpha level, and therefore it is possible that some of the significant correlations here represent type I errors.
This study examined relationships between hypomanic symptoms and impulsivity and risk-taking behaviours in an international sample of undergraduate students, finding that the two are related, with specific aspects of impulsivity and risk-taking being related to specific hypomanic symptoms. This study has a number of limitations, but suggests that hypomania is related to impulsivity and risk-taking propensity in non clinical populations. These findings give insight into the potentially negative consequences of hypomania, such as foolish business investments and spending sprees, in line with DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria, and previous research. Future research should use structured clinical interviews and examine the relationships in clinical bipolar populations, and should also try to clarify whether impulsivity and hypomania are related via trait or state characteristics.
Akiskal, H, S. & Pinto O. (1999). The evolving bipolar spectrum. Prototypes I, II, III, and IV. Psychiatr Clin North Am, 22, 517–534.
American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
Angst, J., Adolfsson, R., Benazzi, F., Gamma, A., Hantouche, E., Meyer, T. D., Skeppar, P., Vieta, E. & Scott, J. (2005a). The HCL-32: Towards a self-assessment tool for hypomanic symptoms in outpatients. J Affect Disord, 88, 217–233.
Angst, J., Sellaro, R., Stassen, H. H. & Gamma, A. (2005b). Diagnostic conversion from depression to bipolar disorders: results of a long-term prospective study of hospital admissions. J Affect Disord, 84, 149–157.
Angst, J. (1998). The emerging epidemiology of hypomania and bipolar II disorder. J Affect Disord, 50, 143-151.
Arnold, L. M. (2003). Gender differences in bipolar disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am, 26(3), 595-620.
Barratt, E.S. (2000). Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, version 11 (BIS-11). In Rush, A. J., Pincus, H. A., First, M. B., Blacker, D., Endicott, J. & Keith, S. J. (Eds.): Handbook of psychiatric measures, p.691-693. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.
Benazzi, F. (2008). A relationship between bipolar II disorder and borderline personality disorder? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry, 32(4), 1022-1029.
Benazzi, F. (2007a). Impulsivity in bipolar-II disorder: Trait, state, or both? Eur Psychiatry, 22(7), 472-478.
Blais, A-R. & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judg Dec Making, 1(1), 33-47.
Chapman, L. J., Champan, J. P., Numbers, J. S., Edell, W. S., Carpenter, B. N. & Beckfield, D. (1984). Impulsive nonconformity as a trait contributing to the prediction of psychotic-like and schziotypal symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis, 172(11), 681-91.
D’Acremont, M. & Van Der Linden, M. (2005). Adolescent impulsivity: findings from a community sample. J Youth Adoles, 34(5), 427-435.
Dell'Osso, L., Carmassi, C., Carlini, M., Rucci, P., Stat, D., Torri, P., Cesari, D., Landi, P., Ciapparelli, A. & Maggi, M. (2009). Sexual Dysfunctions and Suicidality in Patients with Bipolar Disorder and Unipolar Depression. J Sex Medicine, 6(11), 3063-3070.
Durbin, C. E., Schalet, B. D., Hayden, E. P., Simpson, J. & Jordan, P. L. (2009). Hypomanic personality traits: A multi-method exploration of their association with normal and abnormal dimensions of personality. J Res Personality, 43, 898–905.
Eckblad, M. & Chapman, L. J. (1986). Development and validation of a scale for hypomanic personality. J Abnorm Psycholo, 95, 214–222.
Holmes, M. K., Bearden, C. E., Barguil, M., Fonseca, M., Monkul, E. S., Nery, F. G., Soares, J. C., Mintz, J. & Glahn, D. C. (2009). Conceptualizing impulsivity and risk taking in bipolar disorder: importance of history of alcohol abuse. Bipolar Disord, 11, 33–40.
Kawa, I., Carter, J. D., Joyce, P. R., Doughty, C. J., Frampton, C. M., Wells, J. E., Walsh, A. E. S. & Olds, R. J. (2005). Gender differences in bipolar disorder: age of onset, course, comorbidity, and symptom presentation. Bipolar Disord, 7, 119–125.
Kwapil, T. R., Miller, M. B., Zinser, M. C., Chapman, L. J., Chapman, J. & Eckblad, M. (2000). A Longitudinal Study of High Scorers on the Hypomanic Personality Scale. J Abnorm Psycholo, 109(2), 222-226.
Lewis, M., Scott, J. & Frangou, S. (2009). Impulsivity, personality and bipolar disorder. Eur Psychiatry, 24, 464–469.
McCormick, R. A., Russo, A. M., Ramirez, L. F. & Taber, J. I. (1984). Affective disorders among pathological gamblers seeking treatment. Am J Psychiatry, 141, 215-218.
McElroy, S. L., Pope, H. G., Keck, P. E., Hudson, J. I., Phillips, K. A. & Strakowski, S.M. (1996). Are Impulse-control disorders related to bipolar disorder? Compr Psychiatry, 37(4), 229-240.
McElroy, S. L., Pope, H. G., Hudson, J., Keck, P. E. & White, K. L. (1991). Kleptomania: a report of 20 cases. Am J Psychiatry, 148, 652-657.
Michaelis, B. H., Goldberg, J. F., Davis, G. P., Singer, T. M., Garno, J. L. & Wenze, S. J. (2004). Dimensions of impulsivity and aggression associated with suicide attempts among bipolar patients: a preliminary study. Suicide Life Threat Behav, 34(2), 172-176.
Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M. & Swann, A.C. (2001). Psychiatric Aspects of Impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry, 158, 1783–1793.
Peluso, M. A., Hatch, J. P., Glahn, D. C., Monkul, E. S., Sanches, M., Najt, P., Bowden, C. L., Barratt, E. S. & Soares, J. C. (2007). Trait impulsivity in patients with mood disorders. J Affect Disord, 100(1-3), 227-31.
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S. & Woolard, J. (2008). Age Differences in Sensation Seeking and Impulsivity as Indexed by Behavior and Self-Report: Evidence for a Dual Systems Model. Dev Psycholo, 44(6), 1764–1778.
Swann, A. C., Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Steinberg, J. L. & Moeller, F. G. (2009a). Increased trait-like impulsivity and course of illness in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disord, 11(3), 280–288.
Swann, A. C., Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Steinberg, J. L. & Moeller, F. G. (2009b). Severity of bipolar disorder is associated with impairment of response inhibition. J Affect Disord, 116, 30–36.
Swann, A. C., Steinberg, J. L., Lijffijt, M. & Moeller, F. G. (2008). Impulsivity: differential relationship to depression and mania in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord, 106(3), 241-248.
Swann, A. C., Moeller, F. G., Steinberg, J. L., Schneider, L., Barrat, E. S. & Dougherty, D. M. (2007). Manic symptoms and impulsivity during bipolar depressive episodes. Bipolar Disord, 9(13), 206-212.
Swann, A. C., Dougherty, D. M., Pazzaglia, P. J., Pham, M., Steinberg, J. L. & Moeller, F. G. (2005). Increased impulsivity associated with severity of suicide attempt history in patients with bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry, 162, 1680-1687.
Swann, A. C., Dougherty, D. M., Pazzaglia, P. J., Pham, M. & Moeller, F.G. (2004). Impulsivity: a link between bipolar disorder and substance abuse. Bipolar Disord, 6, 204–212.
Swann, A. C., Pazzaglia, P., Nicholls, A., Dougherty, D. M. & Moeller, F. G. (2003). Impulsivity and phase of illness in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord, 73(1), 105-111.
Swann, A. C., Anderson, J. C., Dougherty, D. M. & Moeller, F. G. (2001a). Measurement of inter-episode impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res, 101, 195–197.
Swann, A. C., Janicak, P. L., Calabrese, J. R., Bowden, C. L., Dilsaver, S. C., Morris, D. D., Petty, F. & Davis, L. L. (2001b). Structure of mania: subgroups with distinct clinical characteristics and course of illness in randomized clinical trial participants. J Affect Disord, 67(1-3), 123-132.
Taylor, C. T., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Ostacher, M. J., Chow, C. W., LeBeau, R. T., Pollack, M. H., Nierenberg, A. A. & Simon, N. M. (2008). Anxiety is associated with impulsivity in bipolar disorder. J Anxiety Disord, 22, 868–876.
Vieta, E., Sanchez-Moreno, J., Bulbena, A., Chamorro, L., Ramos, J. L., Artal, J., Perez, F., Oliveras, M. A., Valle, J., Lahuerta, J. & Angst, J. (2007). Cross validation with the mood disorder questionnaire (MDQ) of an instrument for the detection of hypomania in Spanish: The 32 item hypomania symptoms check list (HCL-32). J Affect Disord, 101(2-7), 43-55.
|