Parent-Toddler Play Interaction and its Relation to the Home Environment
Erika Welch, Bronwyn Fees*, and Ann D. Murray*
Kansas State University
Erika is a senior at Kansas State University with a dual major in family
studies and human services and social work. Her professional interests lie in
working with parents and their infants. She will complete her program of study
in the summer of 2004.
This research has been approved by the Kansas State University Institutional
Review Board on Human Subjects.
Abstract
The purpose of this study was to identify patterns in play interaction
between parents and toddlers, and to examine the relationship of these patterns
to aspects of their home environments. The subjects were enrolled in full day
child care, including an Early Head Start program. Home environments were
assessed using the HOME Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001), and parents
and their children (n = 10) were observed in structured play sessions
when the children reached 20 or 30 months of age. Results indicated a
significant correlation between the responsivity subscale of the HOME and both
elaborativeness and sensitivity during play interaction, providing evidence to
support that the HOME scale is a valid indicator of parent-child interaction.
Parent-Infant Play Interaction and its Relation to the Home Environment
Parent-child interaction is a topic that has been widely researched in a
variety of settings, revealing observations that encompass many aspects of
parenting and parent-child relationships. Many of these findings indicate a
need for further research in the area of parent-child interaction, which is
fundamental to the growth and development of children, who learn through
contextual influences (Berk & Winsler, 1995). Research examining play
interaction in relation to home environments contributes to a greater
understanding of which social contexts allow children to grow in their
zone of proximal development (what a child can do with help and
what can be done without guidance) and promote optimum cognitive development in
preparation for future challenges (Berk & Winsler).
Interaction Style and Child Outcomes
Parents styles of play interaction are believed to affect the overall
quality of interaction and therefore the quality of parent-child relationships.
Although many factors influence interaction, one topic that has been widely
studied is the role of the internal state of the parent. Negative internal
symptoms could be the result of factors such as lack of resources or family
stress, which contribute to the social context of child development. Even if
these factors are not present, parental mental health issues affect the quality
of interaction with children.
Parental directiveness is one aspect of parent-child interaction that could
be significantly influenced by a parents internal state. Examining
parental directiveness is essential in studying parent-child interaction,
because the extent to which a parent guides a childs actions is a
defining trait in examining their overall style. A longitudinal study by
Marchand, Hock, & Widaman (2002) examined the relations between maternal
depressive symptoms and hostile-controlling behavior during play
interaction. Researchers found maternal behavior was not related to control.
However, mothers depressive symptoms did affect childrens behavior
in certain contexts. Depressive symptoms were related to childrens
internalizing behaviors (feelings of inferiority, depression, or worthlessness)
at age 4, and maternal depressive symptoms at this age predicted externalizing
behaviors (temper tantrums, threats, impulsive acts, etc.) at age 6, implying
the impact of early maternal patterns on later child development and
specifically indicating that a parents style of play interaction has an
effect on child outcomes (Marchand et al.).
In a similar study, Martin, Clements, & Crnic (2002) examined maternal
emotions during interaction in specific contexts. In this case, the subjects
were observed during free play as well as a more challenging waiting
task that was designed to create a stressful situation for both the
children and their mothers. The results showed no correlation between
mothers reported and expressed emotions during the play period,
indicating that mothers may hide negative emotions to promote childrens
cooperation during play. However, researchers also found correlations between
childrens expressed emotions and mothers self-reported emotions
during play interaction. Therefore, it appears that the emotional experiences
of mothers during play interaction are shaped in part by the
effectiveness with which the dyad is able to negotiate the emotional demands of
such interactions (Martin et al.). However, the correlations do not
indicate directionality of emotional influences, so it is unclear whether the
children internalized the emotions of their mothers or whether the
mothers styles of interaction were influenced by the emotions of their
children. It is likely that the influences were bi-directional (Martin et al.).
From a socio-cultural perspective, it seems that the development of the ability
to handle emotionally demanding situations comes with social interaction, in
which children, as active agents in their development, gradually internalize
these behaviors.
Maternal Sensitivity
A second area that has been widely studied is the specific role of maternal
sensitivity during play. Crawley and Spiker (1983) define sensitivity as
The degree to which the mothers behavior is in tune with that of
the child; the degree to which the mothers behavior reflects awareness of
her childs cues or signals. Parental sensitivity during play
interaction is a logical indicator of healthy parent-child relationships; if
parents are receptive to their childrens cues during play, it follows
that they would demonstrate this awareness across other contexts. Parental
sensitivity became a focus of study as a result of Ainsworth &
Wittigs (1969) research on attachment through observations of the
behaviors of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Ainsworth (1971)
developed a scale to assess maternal sensitivity based on these observations.
This scale has been widely used as a basis for research in this area. In one
study by Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley, & Tuckey (2001), Ainsworths
maternal sensitivity scale was used as an additional measure of dimensions of
sensitivity. Specifically, this study analyzed the relations between
traditional measures of sensitivity and the modern concept of
mind-mindedness, defined as the parental ability to view a child as
an individual with a mind rather than a simple creature with demands to be met
(Meins et al.). Maternal sensitivity as measured using Ainsworths scale
strongly predicted future security of attachment, but
mind-mindedness as a whole did not, providing strong evidence for a
distinction between sensitive and mind-minded maternal
behaviors. However, the category of appropriate mind-related
comments as a subset of mind-mindedness was a stronger predictor of
attachment than maternal sensitivity (Meins et al.). These findings indicate
the possible need for future studies in parent-child interaction to include an
analysis of maternal mind-mindedness as a separate measure from
maternal sensitivity.
Timmer, Borrego, & Urquiza (2002) compared patterns of maternal
responsiveness in physically abusive and non-abusive mother-child dyads.
Findings of interest showed that, although both groups of mothers asked about
the same amount of questions, when the children did not respond, abusive
mothers were three times more likely to follow with a command. Abusive mothers
were also more likely to respond negatively to their childrens
acknowledgements (Timmer et al.). Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, London, & Cabrera
(2002) observed the relationship between father-child interactions and
cognitive development in low-income children and found that high levels of
responsiveness, especially in combination with high language quality, were
associated with quality play and higher-level communication in children
(Shannon et al.). Together, these studies indicate the importance of parental
awareness of childrens needs and appropriate responsiveness to cues in
quality interaction and healthy parent-child relationships.
Overall, research indicates that parental styles of interaction,
specifically parental directiveness and sensitivity, play an important role in
the quality of interaction. Furthermore, the ability of a parent to be aware of
a childs needs and act on them appropriately while handling their
emotional states is a crucial factor in quality interaction. Such active
engagement in problem solving allows children to internalize the mental
processes necessary for healthy cognitive development (Berk & Winsler,
1995).
Environmental Influences
Another widely studied topic in relation to parent-child interaction is the
contribution of environmental factors to interaction behaviors. The home is the
primary context of influence, especially during infancy and toddlerhood, and it
is in this context that the scaffolding (gradual attainment of
skills) of early learning ideally takes place. Also, it is possible that
particular home environments promote quality parent-child interaction, and the
home environment could be predictive of child outcomes. Berk and Winsler (1995)
stress the impact of physical environment in shaping the opportunities
available for play.
Research examining environmental influences (Weinfield, Ogawa, &
Egeland, 2002) has assessed the predictability of mother-child interactive
behaviors over time. This study revealed that outside risk factors such as low
maternal literacy, child behavior problems, and childs general health,
are related to higher predictability of mother-child interaction patterns
(Weinfield et al.). In other words, when these risk factors are present,
certain interaction patterns are more likely to remain from the time the child
is preschool-aged until middle childhood. Such research emphasizes the
importance of the link between interaction and background factors, including
the home environment. It is essential to increase understanding of the
relationship between background circumstances and interaction quality; each is
influenced by the other, and the two combine to form a large amount of the
social context for active learning in developing children.
The focus of our research was on the relationships between parent-child
interaction and home environments. Specifically, we questioned whether there
are correlations between parental directiveness, elaborativeness, and
sensitivity during interaction and responsivity, acceptance, organization,
learning materials, involvement, and variety in the home environment. Research
indicates that parental behavior during play interaction has an effect on child
outcomes. Furthermore, the home environment is the primary context for
development in infants and toddlers who are not able to consciously leave the
physical and emotional surroundings that have been created by their parents.
Therefore, it is logical that qualities of the home environment would be highly
related to qualities of interaction, because parents are involved in both
contexts. Specifically, we hypothesized that elaborativeness and sensitivity
would positively correlate with responsivity, acceptance, and involvement;
these dimensions are related in that they all address parent-child relations
and parental awareness of childrens needs. We also hypothesized that
parental directiveness would negatively correlate with acceptance in the home
environment because of the differing nature of these two qualities. The
directiveness scale assesses the extent to which parents give commands and take
the lead in play interaction (Crawley & Spiker, 1983), and the acceptance
subscale of the HOME assesses the parents ability to avoid unneccessarily
restricting the child (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001).
Method
Sample
The sample consisted of ten children from a rural midwestern background who
were each enrolled in one of two full-day childcare programs. One of these
included children in Early Head Start, a program for economically disadvantaged
children. The other program enrolled children across the economic levels. Of
the ten participants, one child was African American, one was bilingual, and
the remaining eight were of European descent. Families were participating in a
longitudinal study called KITES (Kansas Infant and Toddler Environmental
Study), a pilot study investigating the relations between infant development
and parent-child interaction over time, from birth to 36 months.
Children enter the study at any of nine data collection points within this
age range and participate until they reach the exit point at 36
months. Upon entry, a home visit was conducted, and parents were interviewed
using the HOME Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001) to assess each
childs living environment.
Following the initial interview, children and their parents were observed in
a structured play interaction when the child was either 20 or 30 months of age.
The gender ratio among the children was equal (n = 5 female). There was
some variety, however, among the caregivers participating in the study. A
grandparent accompanied one child, three were observed with their mothers, two
were observed with their fathers, and the remaining four children had both
parents present during the observation.
Measures
Home Environment. The HOME Assessment (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001)
is a tool that has been widely used in child development research. Since the
home is the primary environment for young children, it is the most logical
place to begin any assessment of environmental factors (Caldwell &
Bradley). After completing a training process using sample videotaped
interviews, two researchers completed a home visit for each new participant in
the study. One of these researchers asked questions, while the other observed
and coded to provide reliability.
The HOME assessment includes 45 items divided into six categories. The first
subscale, responsivity, assesses the extent to which the parent appropriately
acknowledged the childs behavior. Acceptance focuses on the parents
ability to cope with undesirable behaviors while avoiding unnecessary
restriction or punishment. Organization refers to the regularity of the
familys schedule, the safety of the physical environment, and the use of
community resources. The assessment of learning materials determines whether
the child is provided with a stimulating environment that includes
developmentally appropriate toys and activities. Involvement refers to whether
the parent takes an active role in their childs cognitive development and
is sensitive to their needs as they mature. Finally, variety determines whether
the child is provided with opportunities for diverse experiences in their daily
life without experiencing disorganization (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001). Each
subscale lists specific items to attend to during the visit, and one point is
given for each listed item that is observed in the home. (Caldwell &
Bradley). The points are totaled for each subscale, and then the subscales are
totaled for an overall score out of a possible 45. Caldwell and Bradley have
created a scale to use in assessing these totals to determine whether a home
falls into the lowest fourth, middle half, or
upper fourth of homes assessed, with higher scores indicating safer
home environments that promote healthier child development.
Parent-Child Interaction. The Mother-Child Rating Scales developed by
Crawley and Spiker (1983) were used to code videotaped observations of
parent-child play interaction. This instrument is divided into two sections:
child ratings (play maturity, social initiative, and object initiative) and
maternal ratings (directiveness, elaborativeness, and sensitivity). Each
characteristic is assessed on a 5-point scale (1 = low, 5 = high).
Directiveness is defined as the degree to which the parent attempts to guide
the childs behavior. Elaborativeness refers to the extent to which the
parent follows and extends the childs self-initiated behaviors.
Sensitivity is defined as the parents awareness of the childs cues
and signals and whether the parents behavior is in accordance with that
of the child (Crawley & Spiker).
Procedure
Home assessment. An initial home visit, when each participant entered
the study, was conducted by two researchers who had each undergone the training
process for the HOME assessment (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001). In each visit,
one researcher conducted the interview while the other observed and coded to
establish reliability. Each visit consisted of a warm up, in which
the researchers spent a few minutes acquainting themselves with the parent and
key child, then an interview was conducted with the tone of two friendly
people who like to talk about children sitting down together and doing exactly
that (Caldwell & Bradley). Inter-rater reliability was examined using
50% of the cases, and agreement was reached on 98.5% of responses.
Parent-child interaction. Parents and their children were invited to
participate in a structured play session in a playroom at a university-based
lab school. At the beginning of each ten-minute videotaped play period,
a standard set of age-appropriate toys were presented, including a doll with a
blanket, a bus with three miniature riders, a pull dog toy, a telephone, a
jack-in-the-box, a push car, a plastic container with a screw lid with a foam
ball inside, a board book, a tea set, a brush, and a comb. The parent or
parents present were instructed to play with their children as they normally
would at home. At the end of each session, the parents and children were asked
to help each other put the toys away. As an expression of appreciation for
their participation, the parents received a small cash award, and each child
received an age-appropriate book.
Each videotape was coded by a trained researcher using Crawley and
Spikers (1983) scales. Each viewing session consisted of three separate
viewings of the tape. The first viewing was for overall familiarity, the second
viewing was used to code for child measures, and the third viewing was for
parental measures. Two researchers, one of whom was blind to the purpose of the
study, coded six of the ten tapes to establish reliability. Three of these six
tapes were viewed together a second time to discuss variations in scores and to
reach consensus for data entry. Among the tapes that were coded by both
researchers, 100% interrater reliability was established within one point on
the scale for each item.
Results
The purpose of our study was to examine the relationships between parental
directiveness, elaborativeness, and sensitivity during play interaction and the
qualities of the childs home environment. According to Vygotskys
sociocultural theory, social interaction mediates the learning process (Berk
& Winsler, 1995), and examining parental characteristics during play
interaction increases our understanding of the factors that affect child
outcomes. Furthermore, environmental factors contribute to the context in which
this interaction takes place. Together, the home environment and the quality of
parent-child interaction create a large part of the childs social climate
in which internalization of mental processes takes place.
Pearson product moment correlations analyses were used to examine the degree
of variance shared between parental directiveness, elaborativeness, and
sensitivity during interaction and the six subscales of the HOME assessment
(See Table 1). A significant positive correlation (r = .93, p
< .01) was found between parental elaborativeness and parental sensitivity,
indicating that, within our sample, the parents who followed their
childrens self-initiated behaviors and creatively extended on
child-initiated play were also aware of their childrens signals and cues.
However, directiveness was not related to either of the other parental
measures. The absence of positive correlations between directiveness and either
of the other two measures suggests that parents who have more directive
interaction styles are not necessarily more sensitive and do not necessarily
elaborate on their childrens leads more often than other parents.
Table 1
Correlations Between HOME Subscales and Parenting Characteristics
Variables
|
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
5
|
6
|
7
|
8
|
9
|
10
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
1. HOME Responsivity
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
2. HOME Acceptance
|
-.56
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
3. HOME Organization
|
.16
|
.39
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
4. HOME Learning Materials
|
.45
|
-.25
|
.65*
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
5. HOME Involvement
|
-.24
|
.54
|
.26
|
-.32
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
6. HOME Variety
|
.33
|
.32
|
.71*
|
.30
|
.21
|
|
|
|
|
|
7. HOME Total
|
.29
|
.48
|
.87**
|
.39
|
.52
|
.86**
|
|
|
|
|
8. Parental Directiveness
|
-.57
|
.29
|
-.56
|
-.62
|
-.41
|
-.38
|
-.61
|
|
|
|
9. Parental Elaborativeness
|
.79**
|
-.55
|
.16
|
.43
|
-.06
|
-.08
|
.21
|
-.42
|
|
|
10. Parental Sensitivity
|
.79**
|
-.44
|
.11
|
.39
|
.22
|
.06
|
.30
|
-.51
|
.93**
|
|
* p < .05; **p < .01
Correlation analyses among the six subscales of the HOME assessment revealed
a positive correlation between organization and both variety (r = .71,
p < .05) and learning materials (r = .65, p < .05).
Also, positive correlations were found between total HOME scores and both
organization (r = .87, p < .01) and variety (r = .86,
p < .01).
Correlation between the subscales of the HOME and the parental measures
during interaction revealed a significant positive correlation between the
responsivity subscale and both parental elaborativeness (r = .79,
p < .01) and sensitivity (r = .79, p < .01). This
indicates that, within our sample, parents who demonstrated active engagement
with their children in the home were also more likely to show sensitivity to
their childrens needs and elaborate on child cues during play
interaction.
Discussion and Limitations
As predicted, characteristics of the childs home environment were
correlated with parental characteristics observed in play. Parents active
expansion of their childrens play and sensitive responsiveness during
interaction were related to responsiveness in the home. Sociocultural theory
stresses the importance of scaffolding, or supporting a child in learning new
skills while gradually loosening control, allowing the child to work toward
independent competence (Berk & Winsler, 1995). It is logical that parents
who show responsiveness in the home environment would have a greater
sensitivity to a childs needs, including their skill level, and the
appropriate amount of support to provide during play interaction. Such parents,
therefore, would likely score highly on elaborativeness, or extending on a
childs leads during play, as well as sensitivity, or awareness of the
childs needs and cues during interaction.
The correlations between responsiveness in the home and both elaborativeness
and sensitivity during interaction demonstrate that, in our study, parents who
responded appropriately to childrens needs in the home environment also
did so in a structured play setting. This finding suggests that parents who
demonstrate sensitivity and awareness are able to practice these habits in
environments away from the home. Further research could test this belief by
assessing parent-child interaction in alternative environments in comparison
with the home or structured play setting.
One important implication of this finding is the possibility that the
responsiveness subscale of the HOME assessment could be an indicator of
parental elaborativeness and sensitivity during interaction. That is, the
responsivity subscale alone could be sufficient to ascertain certain qualities
of parent-child play interaction. Further research is needed to support this
assumption.
Other hypotheses were not supported in our findings. Scores on the HOME
subscales of acceptance and involvement did not significantly correlate with
scores for elaborativeness and sensitivity. This could be explained by the
differing content of the measures. Acceptance in the HOME deals with how the
parent copes when the childs behavior is less than optimal, and
involvement determines the extent to which the parent is engaged in the
childs learning (Caldwell & Bradley, 2001). Although it seems logical
that parents who demonstrate such patterns in the home would also show
elaborative and sensitive interaction patterns, the context of the home is very
different from the observed interaction setting, and the parental measures
during interaction deal specifically with a structured play session (Crawley
& Spiker, 1983).
Furthermore, there was not a significant negative correlation between
directiveness during interaction and acceptance in the home. Directive parents
are not necessarily more likely to practice strict discipline such as scolding,
criticizing, slapping, or spanking in the home. It is possible that
directiveness during play interaction is not related to whether a parent shows
acceptance of their child in the home environment. Clearly, further research is
needed to examine the specific relationship between directive parenting styles
and parental acceptance.
The purpose of a pilot study is to test both methods and relationships
between variables in order to affirm the presence of relationships indicating
the need for further study. This was a pilot study, and our sample size was
small (n = 10). Furthermore, the sample was not representative of the
community. However, our findings revealed interesting trends that merit further
study in the area of parent-child interaction in relation to the home
environment. From a sociocultural perspective, this relationship is essential
in understanding the social context in which children are growing and learning.
Further understanding of this connection could potentially provide areas in
which to focus intervention to promote optimum social contexts for children to
internalize the skills and behaviors necessary to gain competence and
preparation for later challenges.
Clearly, the relationship between environmental factors and quality of
parent-child interaction is a complicated topic requiring much further
research. From a Vygotskian perspective, it is an area that cannot be ignored
because providing a positive home environment is one way that parents
scaffold, or support their childrens gradual attainment of
independent skills. Parent-child interaction is influenced by a variety of
factors, and background circumstances can be key determinants of healthy
connections between children and parents. The consistency observed between the
home environment and structured play suggests that the play session alone may
be a strong indicator of responsiveness in the home environment. Some parental
behaviors were consistent across both contexts, and these relationships deserve
further examination.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1971). Individual
differences in Strange Situation behavior of one year olds. In H. R. Schaffer
(Ed.), The origins of human social relations. New York: Academic Press.
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969). Attachment and exploratory
behavior of one year olds in a strange situation. In B. M. Foss (Ed.),
Determinants of Infant Behavior Vol. 4. New York: Barnes and Noble.
Berk, L. E., & Winsler, A. (1995). Scaffolding childrens
learning: Vygotsky and early childhood education. Washington, D. C.:
National Association for the Education of Young Children.
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (2001). HOME Inventory administration
manual, third edition. University of Arkansas at Little Rock: Print Design
Crawley, S. B., & Spiker, D. (1983). Mother-child rating scales.
University of Illinois at Chicago: Author.
Marchand, J. F., Hock, E., & Widaman, K. (2002). Mutual relations
between mothers depressive symptoms and hostile-controlling behavior in
young childrens externalizing and internalizing behavior problems.
Parenting: Science and Practice, 2(4), 335-353.
Martin, S. E., Clements, M. L., & Crnic, K. A. (2002). Maternal emotions
during mother-toddler interaction: Parenting in affective context.
Parenting: Science and Practice, 2(2), 105-126.
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking
maternal sensitivity: Mothers comments on infants mental processes
predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 42(5), 647-648.
Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., London, K., & Cabrera, N. (2002).
Beyond rough and tumble: Low-income fathers interactions and
childrens cognitive development at 24 months. Parenting: Science and
Practice, 2(2), 77-104.
Timmer, S. G., Borrego, J., & Urquiza, A. J. (2002). Antecedents of
coercive interactions in physically abusive mother-child dyads. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 17(8), 836-853.
Weinfield, N. S., Ogawa, J. R., & Egeland, B. (2002). Predictability of
observed mother-child interaction from preschool to middle childhood in a
high-risk sample. Child Development, 73(2), 528-543.